Comprehensive Physiology Wiley Online Library

Neuronal Plasticity and the Modification of Behavior

Full Article on Wiley Online Library



Abstract

The sections in this article are:

1 Strategies in Neuronal Studies of Behavioral Modifications
1.1 Dynamic and Plastic Capabilities of Synaptic Pathways
1.2 Plasticity Hypothesis
1.3 Dynamic Hypothesis
2 Synaptic Mechanisms of Plastic Change: Potentiation and Depression
2.1 Posttetanic Potentiation: Enhancement of Synaptic Transmission Following Use
2.2 Tenotomy: Enhancement of Synaptic Transmission Through Disuse
2.3 Depression of Synaptic Pathways Following Use
2.4 Plastic Capabilities of Electrical and Chemical Synapses
2.5 Plastic Capabilities of the Chemical Synapses of Invertebrates
2.6 Heterosynaptic Facilitation, Depression, and Sprouting
3 Nonsynaptic Mechanisms of Plastic Change
4 Biochemical Aspects of Neuronal Plasticity
5 Morphological Aspects of Neuronal Plasticity
6 Relation of Neuronal Plasticity to Behavioral Modifications
7 Cellular Studies of Behavioral Modifications in the Immature Nervous System
7.1 Development of Connections in the Visual System
7.2 Effects of Light and Pattern Deprivation in Newborn Animals on Functional Interconnections of Cells in the Visual System
8 Cellular Studies of Behavioral Modifications in Mature Animals
8.1 Habituation and Dishabituation
8.2 Flexion Withdrawal in the Cat
8.3 Gill Withdrawal in Aplysia
8.4 Escape Swimming in Crayfish
8.5 Escape Response in the Cockroach
8.6 Short‐Term Synaptic Plasticity and Short‐Term Habituation
9 Mechanistic Relationships Among Simple Behavioral Modifications
9.1 Relationship of Habituation to Dishabituation
9.2 Relationship of Short‐ to Long‐term Memory
9.3 Relationship of Long‐term Sensitization to Behavioral Abnormalities
9.4 Relationship of Habituation to Higher Forms of Learning
10 Complex Behavioral Modifications: Classical Conditioning
10.1 Classical Conditioning in the Isolated Spinal Cord
10.2 Classical Conditioning of the Eye‐blink Response
10.3 Classical Conditioning in Simple Invertebrates: Feeding in Gastropod Molluscs
11 Overview
Figure 1. Figure 1.

Posttetanic potentiation of the monosynaptic spinal reflex in the cat. A: monosynaptic reflex discharge from gastrocnemius motor neurons in response to a maximum group I afferent volley from gastrocnemius nerve. Control responses (1–4) were followed by a 12‐s tetanus at 555/s, which led to the development of a facilitation of the reflex response (6–8) that persisted for slightly longer than 2 min (b‐i). Test stimuli are presented at 2.4‐s intervals. B: intracellular potentials recorded from biceps semitendinosus motor neuron and generated by single volleys in the biceps semitendinosus nerve. Following a tetanus of 450/s for 15 s, the postsynaptic potential was first depressed for about 0.5–1 s and then potentiated for the next 1.5 min. Test interval in seconds is shown at bottom of each record. The potentiated excitatory postsynaptic potential became large enough to trigger a spike. C: prolonged posttetanic potentiation following 20‐min tetanus. Stimulus was applied to tibial nerve; recordings were obtained from L7 ventral root. •, from left (test) side; ○, from right (control) side which was treated exactly the same as test side except tetanus was omitted. Each plotted point represents an average of 10–20 individual responses. Each test stimulus was delivered at 10‐s intervals. The ordinate refers to degree of potentiation compared to a control value of 1.

A, from Lloyd 159; B, from Eccles 57; C, from Spencer & April 225
Figure 2. Figure 2.

Long‐lasting posttetanic potentiation of the hippocampus. A: 1, diagrammatic view of a parasagittal section of the hippocampal formation showing a stimulating electrode placed beneath the angular bundle (ab) to activate perforant pathway fibers (PP) and recording microelectrode in the molecular layer of the dentate area (AD). Hipp Fiss, hippocampal fissure; Stim, stimulatory electrode; Rec, recording electrode; Fim, fimbrial. 2, arrangement of electrodes for concomitant stimulation of experimental pathway and control pathway (in the contralateral hippocampus). B: amplitude of the population excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) for the experimental pathway (•) and ipsilateral control pathway (○) as a function of time and of conditioning trains (15/s for 10 s) indicated by arrows. Each value is a computed average of 30 responses. Values are plotted as a percentage of the mean preconditioning value of the population (POP) EPSP.

From Bliss & Lømo 21
Figure 3. Figure 3.

Posttetanic potentiation in neuromuscular preparation treated with tetrodotoxin. End‐plate potential (EPP) recorded from a single junction before and after tetanic stimulation applied to the motor nerve terminal. Pulses were 1.0 ms in duration and 3.5 μA in intensity. A: sample records of EPP's before (C) and at indicated intervals (in seconds) after the tetanus at 50/s for 20 s. B: time course of potentiation. Each point represents mean amplitude of 5 posttetanic EPP's and refers to time of the 5th response. Ordinate, average amplitude of posttetanic EPP's as a percentage of control EPP amplitude shown by horizontal line. Control EPP amplitude was computed by averaging about 50 pretetanic responses. Discrepancy between sample records (A) and averaged response (B) is due to the EPP amplitude fluctuation. Temperature was 19°C.

From Weinreich 253
Figure 4. Figure 4.

Effects of chronic disuse on synaptic function. A: schematic diagram of the experimental preparation used by Spencer & April 225 to study the effects of disuse (tenotomy) on the synapse between the la afferent fibers and the neurons of Clarke's column nucleus. Interruption of the motor neuron axons (A) by ventral root transection (deefferentation) opens the gamma loop to the tenotomized muscle (B). Note the conventional diagram of the parallel arrangement of intra‐extra‐fusal muscle. In representation of muscle afferents the heavier lines indicate the principal anatomic connections. Dorsal spinocerebellar tract (DSCT) takes its principal connections from the ipsilateral hindlimb group Ia muscle afferents. Ventral spinocerebellar tract (VSCT) takes its principal connections from contralateral hindlimb group Ib muscle afferents. B: monosynaptic (ascending) responses recorded from the acutely transected thoracic spinal cord (1 and 2) and L5 cord dorsum (afferent volley) potential (3 and 4) to single shock stimulation of soleus and gastrocnemius nerves 47 days after unilateral Achilles tenotomy and transection of ventral roots L6, L7, and S1. Traces 1 and 3 are from the operated sides; traces 2 and 4 are from the nonoperated side. Each record consists of 20 superimposed individual responses.

From Spencer & April 225
Figure 5. Figure 5.

Posttetanic depression. Postsynaptic response in the lateral geniculate following tetanization of the contralateral optic nerve in the cat for 20 s at 500/s (arrow). On the ordinate are plotted the postsynaptic responses relative to control. Subnormality was still present 3 h following the 20‐s tetanus.

Slightly modified from Evarts & Hughes 64
Figure 6. Figure 6.

Synaptic depression at low rates of stimulation. A: low‐frequency depression in cat monosynaptic spinal reflex. Relation between afferent stimulus frequency and mean monosynaptic reflex output. Stimulus frequency was varied from 0.05/s (3/ min) to 50/s 160. B: low‐frequency depression of a monosynaptic excitatory post‐synaptic potential in cell R2 of the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia produced by stimulating a single fiber in the connective. The steady‐state value of the unitary postsynaptic potential amplitude is plotted as a function of frequency ranging from 1/10 s to 1/s.

A, replotted from Lloyd & Wilson 160; B, from Kandel & Tauc 127
Figure 7. Figure 7.

Comparison of the plastic capabilities of a chemical and electrical synaptic potential. A: 1, intracellular recording from a cell in chick ciliary ganglion before tetanus. The chemical excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) was reduced in amplitude by adding d‐tubocurarine chloride to bathing solution in a concentration of 5 μg/ml; 2, response recorded 15 s after a tetanus of 50/s was applied to the presynaptic nerve for 20 s; 3–6, response recorded 1, 3, 5, and 10 min after end of tetanus. EPSP is potentiated, whereas electrical synaptic (coupling) potential is unchanged. Cell was hyperpolarized to prevent firing. B: time course of posttetanic potentiation of EPSP in ganglion cell. Measurements of EPSP and electrical coupling potential are expressed as percentage of control amplitude (initial points). Hatched vertical bar shows duration of tetanus (frequency 50/s). EPSP is potentiated by a factor of almost 800%; electrical coupling potential is unchanged.

From Martin & Pilar 170
Figure 8. Figure 8.

Heterosynaptic facilitation. A: intracellular recording from cell R2 in Aplysia abdominal ganglion. 1, test excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) produced by stimulation of genital nerve alone; 2, first of 9 pairing trials of test EPSP and response to priming stimulus (6/s train of 1‐s duration to the siphon nerve); 3, seventh pairing trial (note accompanying depolarization and increases in test PSP); 4–8, test PSP 10 s and 3.5, 10, 20, and 30 min after pairing. B: time course of heterosynaptic facilitation. This is the same preparation as in A but a different run. The amplitude of the test EPSP is plotted as a function of time and of pairing; the period of pairing is indicated by arrows. The graph at the bottom indicates the average level of membrane potential in millivolts.

From Kandel & Tauc 127
Figure 9. Figure 9.

Presynaptic inhibition in Aplysia. Intracellular recording from a red blood cell in the Aplysia abdominal ganglion. 1, complex test excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP's) from the branchial nerve; 2, pairing of test EPSP with a single priming stimulus train (frequency 10/s) to the genital nerve; 3, depression of test EPSP 10 s after priming stimulus; 4–6, recovery of depression during subsequent 4 min.

Figure 10. Figure 10.

Postsynaptic mechanism for heterosynaptic facilitation. A: intracellular recordings from the metacerebral cells of the snail. Dynamic current‐voltage curve obtained directly on the oscilloscope. The transients on the positive voltage axis are the lower parts of the action potentials. Zero represents the resting level (49 mV). B: effects of increasing level of membrane potential on the amplitude and configuration of an excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) and an electrotonic potential produced by a square constant current pulse. C: increase in test EPSP produced by stimulating one pathway as a result of a membrane depolarization produced by stimulating another (primary) pathway. The various frames have been aligned to allow reading of changes in membrane potential. 1, test PSP before stimulation of second pathway; 2, first of 7 stimulus trains to a second pathway; 3, fifth stimulus to the second pathway (note depolarization and increase in test PSP); 4–8, decline of amplitude in test PSP with decline of depolarization in the 6.5 min following stimulus to the other nerve. D: detailed changes in test PSP following a pairing experiment similar to that illustrated in C. (Note changes in the test PSP as the membrane potential declines toward the resting level.) Numbers on the left indicate depolarization (in mV) from the resting level.

From Kandel & Tauc 129
Figure 11. Figure 11.

Synaptic modulations of an endogenously active cell. A: intracellular recordings from cell R15 in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia. Long‐lasting effects of an excitatory synaptic input (INPUT I). 1, sample record from control 25‐min period to illustrate relative constancy of burst size and interval. 2, sample records during stimulation of input I at 10/s for 4 min. This stimulus produced continuous spiking. 3, sample records from 8 min after stimulation was over, burst size stabilized at 27–29 spikes/burst, and the interburst interval was prolonged. 4, sample records after 4 min of hyperpolarizing the cell to block all spiking. The cell first gave a long train of spikes followed by a return to a burst size of 19–20 spikes, and a return of the interburst interval to the control level. 5, sample record during depolarization with outward current to cause continuous spiking for 4 min, paralleling the spiking caused by the synaptic input in 2. 6, sample record following depolarization illustrated in 5. Depolarization did not cause a subsequent change in burst size, but did cause a prolongation of the interburst interval. Calibration; traces 2 and 5, 0.4 s. B: graphic representation of interburst interval, spike frequency with each burst and burst size as modulated by synaptic input (input I). Left (abscissa in minutesi control period to illustrate synaptic regularity of 3 burst parameters over 3 h. The synaptic input (input I) was then stimulated at 6/s for 3 min after which the abscissa was first expressed as consecutive bursts for 40 bursts and then as time beginning with 0. The synaptic input produces an increase in burst size and frequency. The increase in burst size remains increased for 4 h.

From Parnas et al. 184
Figure 12. Figure 12.

Binocular interaction and plasticity in the cat visual system. A: receptive field of a typical neuron in the cat's left visual cortex (area 17) as mapped for the left (1a) and right (2a) eye. The neuron responds with a train of action potentials to a diagonal bright bar moving to the left. The cell responds more effectively when the stimulus is presented to the contralateral eye (2b) than to the ipsilateral eye (1b), B: on the basis of the responses illustrated in A, Hubel and Wiesel divided the response properties of visual cortical cells into 7 ocular dominance groups. If a cell (small circle) in the right hemisphere is influenced only by the contralateral eye, it falls into group 1. If it receives input only from the ipsilateral eye it falls into group 7. For the intermediate groups, one eye may influence the cell much more than the other (groups 2 and 6) or the differences may be slight (groups 3 and 5). According to these criteria, the cell in A would fall into group 2. C: ocular dominance histograms under different experimental conditions. 1, normal adult cat. Histogram based on 233 cells. 2, kittens monocularly deprived of form vision. Histogram based on 199 cells in 5 kittens 8–14 wk old that had their right eye closed by lid suture from the time of normal eye opening. Shading indicates cells that had the usual specific response properties to visual stimulation. Absence of shading indicates cells that lacked the normal orientation specificity. Interrupted lines indicate cells that did not respond to either eye. 3, kittens raised with experimental squint. Histogram based on 384 cells recorded from 4 animals.

A, adapted from Hubel 111 and Blakemore 15; B, adapted from Hubel 111; C, from Hubel and Wiesel 113,114,258
Figure 13. Figure 13.

Critical period for monocular deprivation of pattern vision. Ocular dominance histograms based on individual animals deprived of form vision in one (the right) eye for various periods. Numbers on abscissa refer to ocular dominance grouping (see Fig. 12). A: 2 normal, very young, visually inexperienced kittens — one 8 days old and one 16 days old with no previous pattern vision in either eye. B: monocular deprivation from days 9–19. C: monocular deprivation from days 10–31. D: monocular deprivation from days 23–29. E: monocular deprivation from mo 2–3. F: monocular deprivation from mo 4–7. G: a previously normal adult cat monocularly deprived for 3 mo

Data From Hubel and Wiesel 113,115 and Blakemore 15
Figure 14. Figure 14.

Critical period for reverse suture. A: ocular dominance histograms in animals with reverse suture following earlier eyelid closure. 1, dominance distribution for control animals monocularly deprived in the right eye until 5 wk. 1–6, results from 5 kittens monocularly deprived in the right (ipsilateral) eye until 5 (2), 6 (3), 8 (4), 10 (5), and 14 (6) wks, respectively, and then reverse sutured. In each case the kitten was then allowed 9 wk of vision using its right (ipsilateral) eye following reverse suturing (left eyelids closed) before recording at the age indicated in parentheses. Numbers on abscissa refer to ocular dominance grouping (see Fig. 12). B: time course of the reversibility of the effects of previous eyelid closure. Reversal index is the ratio of the neurons dominated by the more recently experienced eye (7, 6, and 5) to the total number of visually responsive cells. This index provides an indication of the degree to which the initially deprived eye recaptures the cortex. Broken horizontal line shows the same ratio calculated for the control animals that were monocularly deprived until 5 wk and which had no neurons in groups 7, 6, and 5 (index = 0.0).

From Blakemore & Van Sluyters 19
Figure 15. Figure 15.

Habituation and dishabituation in the acute decerebratespinal preparation. Continuous records of isometric myogram of tibialis anterior under low (<50 g) initial tension. Test stimuli were brief trains (0.5‐s duration) of high frequency (50/s) pulses applied to the skin of the rump at 2‐min intervals. A: 1, habituation and spontaneous recovery. Control rate 1/2 min. Period between arrows indicates response decrement following increase in stimulus frequency to 10/s. Recovery follows after 6 stimuli at the control rate of 1/2 min. 2, restoration of the previously decremented flexion reflex (dishabituation) by introduction of a strong stimulus. Test stimuli were similar to those in 1. Response restoration (during period indicated by shaded rectangle) was produced by a brief pinch to digits of same leg. B: synaptic concomitants of habituation in acute spinal animal. Intracellular recordings of polysynaptic excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) decrement from deep peroneal motor neuron (identified by antidromic activation) in response to single shock stimuli delivered to the superficial peroneal nerve. B: 1, during the control period stimulating rate was 2/min; 2, EPSP decrement during increased stimulation rate at 1‐s intervals; 3, restoration of EPSP amplitude following an extra stimulus delivered to the tibial nerve (tetanus at 100/s for 4‐s duration); 4, subsequent EPSP decrement produced by continued stimulation of 1‐s intervals; 5, spontaneous recovery of PSP amplitude when stimulus rate was restored to 1/2 min. Time and voltage calibration from 1–5, 10 ms and 2 mV.

A, from Spencer et al. 228; B, from Spencer et al. 226,227
Figure 16. Figure 16.

Responses of two types of interneurons in the cat spinal cord during habituation and sensitization. A: H interneuron. Upper graph represents the amplitude of the flexor twitch of tibial anterior muscle showing slight increase (sensitization) followed by habituation and spontaneous recovery (following rest). Lower graph represents mean number of spikes per stimulus of a simultaneously recorded interneuron. H interneuron shows only a progressive decrease in the evoked discharge even during behavioral sensitization. B: S interneuron. Upper and lower graphs again illustrate flexor twitch of the tibial anterior muscle showing sensitization followed by habituation and spontaneous recovery. Lower graph represents mean number of spikes per stimulus of the simultaneously recorded interneuron. S interneuron shows an initial increase followed by a decrease in evoked discharge. C: schematic diagram of possible neuronal substrate of habituation and sensitization. N, nonplastic synapses; H, habituating synapses; S, sensitizing synapses. According to this scheme, afferent cutaneous stimuli exert their influence on two systems: a reflex system (S‐R) that mediates habituation and a “state” system that mediates sensitization.

A and C, from Groves & Thompson 96; B, from Groves et al. 95
Figure 17. Figure 17.

Short‐term habituation and dishabituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. A: 1, defensive withdrawal reflex of siphon and gill. Dorsal view of an intact Aplysia. The parapodia and mantle shelf have been retracted to reveal the gill. The most sensitive area of the receptive field for eliciting the withdrawal reflex consists of two parts: the rostral edge of the mantle shelf containing the purple gland (dark area) and the caudal edge of the mantle shelf and its continuation as the siphon. The surrounding area is less sensitive. The position of the mantle organs at rest (dotted line) is compared to that during withdrawal reflex following tactile stimulation of the siphon (solid lines). 2, sample photocell records illustrating habituation, spontaneous recovery, and dishabituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex. The interval between stimuli (ISI) and the total number of habituatory stimuli are indicated. a, decrement of the response with repetition of the stimulus at 3‐min intervals. Following a 122‐min rest, the response was almost fully recovered. b, later experiment from the same preparation. After rehabituation of the response at 1/min, a dishabituatory stimulus consisting of a strong and prolonged tactile stimulus to the neck region was presented at the arrow. Successive responses were facilitated for several minutes. B: central synaptic changes accompanying habituation, recovery, and dishabituation. 1, preparation used to correlate contraction of the gill and responses of motor neuron L7 is similar to that illustrated in A, except that a slit has been made in the neck in order to externalize the abdominal ganglion. To permit intracellular recordings from individual cells, the ganglion, with its nerves intact, has been pinned to a stage. This is illuminated by a light guide. 2–4, the photocell records of the gill contractions are illustrated on the top traces of each line, and the simultaneous intracellular recordings from an identified motor neuron L7 are illustrated on the bottom traces. Sample records are all from the same preparation. Tactile stimuli (500 ms in duration) were presented to the mantle shelf every 90 s. 2, habituation. Stimuli were presented over a period of 21 min. Number of spikes in the 1‐s interval following the first evoked spike in each trace: 9, 6, 6, 4. 3, partial recovery (after a 9‐min rest) and subsequent rehabituation of the reflex. Number of spikes: 7, 6, 5, 3. 4, dishabituation. Following the last habituation trial shown in the first trace, a strong stimulus was applied to the siphon. The discharge of the motor neuron and the amplitude of the gill contraction progressively increased during the first 3 stimuli following the dishabituatory stimulus and remained elevated for several minutes. Number of spikes: 4, 5, 7, 5. C: changes in the elementary monosynaptic excitatory connections between sensory and motor neurons as a result of repetitive stimulation at an interstimulus interval of 1/10 s. 1, experimental setup used to examine elementary connections in the isolated ganglion. Ganglion was bathed in high Mg2+ (160 mM) and Ca2+ (50 mM) solutions. Sens. N, sensory neuron; M.N., motor neuron. 2–4, Intracellular recording from motor neuron L7 (top trace) and sensory neuron (bottom trace). 2, response decrement of elementary excitatory postsynaptic potential in L7 to 15 repeated stimuli of sensory neuron. Only responses to stimulus numbers 1, 2, 6, 10, and 15 are illustrated. 3, a strong stimulus (6/s train for 4 s) was applied to the connective. This stimulus did not alter the firing pattern of the sensory neuron. 4, following the heterosynaptic stimulus to the connective the amplitude of the elementary EPSP was partially restored.

A, from Pinsker et al. 192, copyright 1970 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; B, from Kupfermann et al. 149, copyright 1970 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; C, V. Castellucci and E. R. Kandel, unpublished data
Figure 18. Figure 18.

Estimation of quantal size and quantal content during depression and facilitation of the monosynaptic connection between sensory and motor cells mediating the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Two independent techniques are used: amplitude histogram and failure analysis. A: Amplitude histograms. 200–300 consecutive synaptic responses were obtained by intracellular stimulation of the sensory neuron at 10‐s intervals. The responses were separated into 3 successive plateau regions, each consisting of 30–100 responses in regions of stability in which the EPSP amplitude changed less than 15%. The histograms reveal a peak of failures followed by a multimodal distribution with the mean of each subsequent peak being an integral multiple of the first unit peak. The unit peak was assumed to represent the quantal unit (q). The dotted line and the arrow on the ordinate indicate the theoretical distribution predicted by the Poisson equation. 1, depression. A comparison of the successive regions illustrates that with repeated stimulation the position of the unit peak (I) does not change, but the relative incidence of failures increases 6‐ to 7‐fold from the first to the third region. Thus the 30 stimuli in the first region produced 2 failures, the next 70 stimuli produced 12 failures (or 6 failures for every 35 responses), and the 35 stimuli in the last region produced 14 failures. These findings indicate that the EPSP depression during continued stimulation is due to a decrease in quantal output (in this case m decreased from 4 to 1) while quantal size remains relatively unaffected. 2, facilitation. Synaptic decrement was first produced with 200 consecutive stimuli to the sensory neuron as in 1. The pathway from the head was then stimulated to produce facilitation. The histograms illustrate the last region of depression (prefacilitation region) just before the facilitating stimulus and the 2 regions following the facilitating stimulus (postfacilitation region). In the last region of depression, there are again a large number of failures. Following the facilitating stimulus, the relative number of failures is markedly reduced and changes from 12 failures or 7/33 responses in the first region to 1/33 responses in the next region. But the position of the unit peak again remains the same. Thus, during facilitation, quantal output increases while the size of a quantal unit does not change. B: Failure analysis. Summary of the results on synaptic depression and synaptic facilitation derived by a failure analysis derived from the Poisson equation whereby the quantal content m0 = lnN/n0 where m0 is the quantal content, N is the number of trials and n0 the number of failures. A first group of 5 experiments was done on synaptic depression and was normalized to the first region prior to facilitation. With both depression and facilitation, the estimated values of quantal size remained constant during successive regions while E, the average excitatory postsynaptic potential amplitude, and m, the quantal content, decreased by 50% during the depression and increased by 100% in relation to the control region during facilitation.

Figure 19. Figure 19.

Excitatory postsynaptic potential decrement accompanying habituation in the crayfish. A: lateral giant dendrite responses in the fifth ganglion to repetitive stimulation of the second root of the same segment at 10/s. The 1st, 2nd, 5th, 10th, and 30th responses are shown. The initial downward deflection is the stimulus artifact. The first fast depolarizing potential is an electrotonic component; the slower later wave is the chemically mediated postsynaptic potentials. B: properties of monosynaptic afferent excitatory postsynaptic potentials in tactile interneuron. 1–3, 1st, 4th, and 7th responses to stimulation at 1/5 s. The small late depolarizations are due to occasional activation of tactile afferents whose spikes are not discernible in the root monitor.

From Zucker 274
Figure 20. Figure 20.

Acquisition and retention of long‐term habituation of defensive withdrawal in Aplysia. A: behavioral experiments. Siphon withdrawal is expressed as a percentage of the median of each group's initial response (block 1, trial 1). The median duration of the initial response was 17 s for the experimental and 19 s for the control animals. For statistical analysis, siphon withdrawal for each animal was expressed as a single score: the sum of 10 trials, that is, the total time an animal spent responding in the 10‐trials habituation session. Intergroup statistical comparisons were made with Mann‐Whitney U tests and intragroup comparisons with Wilcoxon matched‐pairs signed‐ranks tests. Following blocks of siphon‐habituation training spaced by 90 min (acquisition), experimental animals (•) exhibited significantly greater habituation (retention) than control animals (○) at 24 h (P < 0.001). B: acquisition and retention of excitatory postsynaptic potential decrement. The EPSP amplitudes from both experimental (•) and control (○) nerves (n = 10) are expressed as a percentage of the initial amplitude. In acquisition, 6 experiments were run with the siphon nerve as experimental. In block 1, 10 stimuli were first applied to the experimental nerve and then to the control nerve, producing in L7 comparable EPSP decrement from both nerves, which indicated lack of EPSP generalization. Repeated blocks of stimuli to the experimental nerve produced progressive buildup of EPSP decrement. A single test to the control nerve produced an EPSP which was recovered to 84.5% control, indicating that deterioration cannot account for experimental EPSP decrement. In retention, the cell was reimpaled 24 h later and repolarized to the membrane potential maintained for acquisition. The ordinate in retention was redrawn to indicate that the repolarization is only closely approximated, it is not exact. In the retention test, stimulation of the experimental nerve produced significantly greater EPSP decrement (P < 0.001) than stimulation of the control nerve. I:b.i., interblock interval; i.t.i., intertrial interval.

Figure 21. Figure 21.

Spinal cord conditioning. Mean amplitudes of the response to the conditioned stimulus (CS) alone from 20 animals for test trials in acquisition and during five trial blocks in extinction, given as a percent of mean CS alone.

From Patterson et al. 187


Figure 1.

Posttetanic potentiation of the monosynaptic spinal reflex in the cat. A: monosynaptic reflex discharge from gastrocnemius motor neurons in response to a maximum group I afferent volley from gastrocnemius nerve. Control responses (1–4) were followed by a 12‐s tetanus at 555/s, which led to the development of a facilitation of the reflex response (6–8) that persisted for slightly longer than 2 min (b‐i). Test stimuli are presented at 2.4‐s intervals. B: intracellular potentials recorded from biceps semitendinosus motor neuron and generated by single volleys in the biceps semitendinosus nerve. Following a tetanus of 450/s for 15 s, the postsynaptic potential was first depressed for about 0.5–1 s and then potentiated for the next 1.5 min. Test interval in seconds is shown at bottom of each record. The potentiated excitatory postsynaptic potential became large enough to trigger a spike. C: prolonged posttetanic potentiation following 20‐min tetanus. Stimulus was applied to tibial nerve; recordings were obtained from L7 ventral root. •, from left (test) side; ○, from right (control) side which was treated exactly the same as test side except tetanus was omitted. Each plotted point represents an average of 10–20 individual responses. Each test stimulus was delivered at 10‐s intervals. The ordinate refers to degree of potentiation compared to a control value of 1.

A, from Lloyd 159; B, from Eccles 57; C, from Spencer & April 225


Figure 2.

Long‐lasting posttetanic potentiation of the hippocampus. A: 1, diagrammatic view of a parasagittal section of the hippocampal formation showing a stimulating electrode placed beneath the angular bundle (ab) to activate perforant pathway fibers (PP) and recording microelectrode in the molecular layer of the dentate area (AD). Hipp Fiss, hippocampal fissure; Stim, stimulatory electrode; Rec, recording electrode; Fim, fimbrial. 2, arrangement of electrodes for concomitant stimulation of experimental pathway and control pathway (in the contralateral hippocampus). B: amplitude of the population excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) for the experimental pathway (•) and ipsilateral control pathway (○) as a function of time and of conditioning trains (15/s for 10 s) indicated by arrows. Each value is a computed average of 30 responses. Values are plotted as a percentage of the mean preconditioning value of the population (POP) EPSP.

From Bliss & Lømo 21


Figure 3.

Posttetanic potentiation in neuromuscular preparation treated with tetrodotoxin. End‐plate potential (EPP) recorded from a single junction before and after tetanic stimulation applied to the motor nerve terminal. Pulses were 1.0 ms in duration and 3.5 μA in intensity. A: sample records of EPP's before (C) and at indicated intervals (in seconds) after the tetanus at 50/s for 20 s. B: time course of potentiation. Each point represents mean amplitude of 5 posttetanic EPP's and refers to time of the 5th response. Ordinate, average amplitude of posttetanic EPP's as a percentage of control EPP amplitude shown by horizontal line. Control EPP amplitude was computed by averaging about 50 pretetanic responses. Discrepancy between sample records (A) and averaged response (B) is due to the EPP amplitude fluctuation. Temperature was 19°C.

From Weinreich 253


Figure 4.

Effects of chronic disuse on synaptic function. A: schematic diagram of the experimental preparation used by Spencer & April 225 to study the effects of disuse (tenotomy) on the synapse between the la afferent fibers and the neurons of Clarke's column nucleus. Interruption of the motor neuron axons (A) by ventral root transection (deefferentation) opens the gamma loop to the tenotomized muscle (B). Note the conventional diagram of the parallel arrangement of intra‐extra‐fusal muscle. In representation of muscle afferents the heavier lines indicate the principal anatomic connections. Dorsal spinocerebellar tract (DSCT) takes its principal connections from the ipsilateral hindlimb group Ia muscle afferents. Ventral spinocerebellar tract (VSCT) takes its principal connections from contralateral hindlimb group Ib muscle afferents. B: monosynaptic (ascending) responses recorded from the acutely transected thoracic spinal cord (1 and 2) and L5 cord dorsum (afferent volley) potential (3 and 4) to single shock stimulation of soleus and gastrocnemius nerves 47 days after unilateral Achilles tenotomy and transection of ventral roots L6, L7, and S1. Traces 1 and 3 are from the operated sides; traces 2 and 4 are from the nonoperated side. Each record consists of 20 superimposed individual responses.

From Spencer & April 225


Figure 5.

Posttetanic depression. Postsynaptic response in the lateral geniculate following tetanization of the contralateral optic nerve in the cat for 20 s at 500/s (arrow). On the ordinate are plotted the postsynaptic responses relative to control. Subnormality was still present 3 h following the 20‐s tetanus.

Slightly modified from Evarts & Hughes 64


Figure 6.

Synaptic depression at low rates of stimulation. A: low‐frequency depression in cat monosynaptic spinal reflex. Relation between afferent stimulus frequency and mean monosynaptic reflex output. Stimulus frequency was varied from 0.05/s (3/ min) to 50/s 160. B: low‐frequency depression of a monosynaptic excitatory post‐synaptic potential in cell R2 of the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia produced by stimulating a single fiber in the connective. The steady‐state value of the unitary postsynaptic potential amplitude is plotted as a function of frequency ranging from 1/10 s to 1/s.

A, replotted from Lloyd & Wilson 160; B, from Kandel & Tauc 127


Figure 7.

Comparison of the plastic capabilities of a chemical and electrical synaptic potential. A: 1, intracellular recording from a cell in chick ciliary ganglion before tetanus. The chemical excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) was reduced in amplitude by adding d‐tubocurarine chloride to bathing solution in a concentration of 5 μg/ml; 2, response recorded 15 s after a tetanus of 50/s was applied to the presynaptic nerve for 20 s; 3–6, response recorded 1, 3, 5, and 10 min after end of tetanus. EPSP is potentiated, whereas electrical synaptic (coupling) potential is unchanged. Cell was hyperpolarized to prevent firing. B: time course of posttetanic potentiation of EPSP in ganglion cell. Measurements of EPSP and electrical coupling potential are expressed as percentage of control amplitude (initial points). Hatched vertical bar shows duration of tetanus (frequency 50/s). EPSP is potentiated by a factor of almost 800%; electrical coupling potential is unchanged.

From Martin & Pilar 170


Figure 8.

Heterosynaptic facilitation. A: intracellular recording from cell R2 in Aplysia abdominal ganglion. 1, test excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) produced by stimulation of genital nerve alone; 2, first of 9 pairing trials of test EPSP and response to priming stimulus (6/s train of 1‐s duration to the siphon nerve); 3, seventh pairing trial (note accompanying depolarization and increases in test PSP); 4–8, test PSP 10 s and 3.5, 10, 20, and 30 min after pairing. B: time course of heterosynaptic facilitation. This is the same preparation as in A but a different run. The amplitude of the test EPSP is plotted as a function of time and of pairing; the period of pairing is indicated by arrows. The graph at the bottom indicates the average level of membrane potential in millivolts.

From Kandel & Tauc 127


Figure 9.

Presynaptic inhibition in Aplysia. Intracellular recording from a red blood cell in the Aplysia abdominal ganglion. 1, complex test excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP's) from the branchial nerve; 2, pairing of test EPSP with a single priming stimulus train (frequency 10/s) to the genital nerve; 3, depression of test EPSP 10 s after priming stimulus; 4–6, recovery of depression during subsequent 4 min.



Figure 10.

Postsynaptic mechanism for heterosynaptic facilitation. A: intracellular recordings from the metacerebral cells of the snail. Dynamic current‐voltage curve obtained directly on the oscilloscope. The transients on the positive voltage axis are the lower parts of the action potentials. Zero represents the resting level (49 mV). B: effects of increasing level of membrane potential on the amplitude and configuration of an excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) and an electrotonic potential produced by a square constant current pulse. C: increase in test EPSP produced by stimulating one pathway as a result of a membrane depolarization produced by stimulating another (primary) pathway. The various frames have been aligned to allow reading of changes in membrane potential. 1, test PSP before stimulation of second pathway; 2, first of 7 stimulus trains to a second pathway; 3, fifth stimulus to the second pathway (note depolarization and increase in test PSP); 4–8, decline of amplitude in test PSP with decline of depolarization in the 6.5 min following stimulus to the other nerve. D: detailed changes in test PSP following a pairing experiment similar to that illustrated in C. (Note changes in the test PSP as the membrane potential declines toward the resting level.) Numbers on the left indicate depolarization (in mV) from the resting level.

From Kandel & Tauc 129


Figure 11.

Synaptic modulations of an endogenously active cell. A: intracellular recordings from cell R15 in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia. Long‐lasting effects of an excitatory synaptic input (INPUT I). 1, sample record from control 25‐min period to illustrate relative constancy of burst size and interval. 2, sample records during stimulation of input I at 10/s for 4 min. This stimulus produced continuous spiking. 3, sample records from 8 min after stimulation was over, burst size stabilized at 27–29 spikes/burst, and the interburst interval was prolonged. 4, sample records after 4 min of hyperpolarizing the cell to block all spiking. The cell first gave a long train of spikes followed by a return to a burst size of 19–20 spikes, and a return of the interburst interval to the control level. 5, sample record during depolarization with outward current to cause continuous spiking for 4 min, paralleling the spiking caused by the synaptic input in 2. 6, sample record following depolarization illustrated in 5. Depolarization did not cause a subsequent change in burst size, but did cause a prolongation of the interburst interval. Calibration; traces 2 and 5, 0.4 s. B: graphic representation of interburst interval, spike frequency with each burst and burst size as modulated by synaptic input (input I). Left (abscissa in minutesi control period to illustrate synaptic regularity of 3 burst parameters over 3 h. The synaptic input (input I) was then stimulated at 6/s for 3 min after which the abscissa was first expressed as consecutive bursts for 40 bursts and then as time beginning with 0. The synaptic input produces an increase in burst size and frequency. The increase in burst size remains increased for 4 h.

From Parnas et al. 184


Figure 12.

Binocular interaction and plasticity in the cat visual system. A: receptive field of a typical neuron in the cat's left visual cortex (area 17) as mapped for the left (1a) and right (2a) eye. The neuron responds with a train of action potentials to a diagonal bright bar moving to the left. The cell responds more effectively when the stimulus is presented to the contralateral eye (2b) than to the ipsilateral eye (1b), B: on the basis of the responses illustrated in A, Hubel and Wiesel divided the response properties of visual cortical cells into 7 ocular dominance groups. If a cell (small circle) in the right hemisphere is influenced only by the contralateral eye, it falls into group 1. If it receives input only from the ipsilateral eye it falls into group 7. For the intermediate groups, one eye may influence the cell much more than the other (groups 2 and 6) or the differences may be slight (groups 3 and 5). According to these criteria, the cell in A would fall into group 2. C: ocular dominance histograms under different experimental conditions. 1, normal adult cat. Histogram based on 233 cells. 2, kittens monocularly deprived of form vision. Histogram based on 199 cells in 5 kittens 8–14 wk old that had their right eye closed by lid suture from the time of normal eye opening. Shading indicates cells that had the usual specific response properties to visual stimulation. Absence of shading indicates cells that lacked the normal orientation specificity. Interrupted lines indicate cells that did not respond to either eye. 3, kittens raised with experimental squint. Histogram based on 384 cells recorded from 4 animals.

A, adapted from Hubel 111 and Blakemore 15; B, adapted from Hubel 111; C, from Hubel and Wiesel 113,114,258


Figure 13.

Critical period for monocular deprivation of pattern vision. Ocular dominance histograms based on individual animals deprived of form vision in one (the right) eye for various periods. Numbers on abscissa refer to ocular dominance grouping (see Fig. 12). A: 2 normal, very young, visually inexperienced kittens — one 8 days old and one 16 days old with no previous pattern vision in either eye. B: monocular deprivation from days 9–19. C: monocular deprivation from days 10–31. D: monocular deprivation from days 23–29. E: monocular deprivation from mo 2–3. F: monocular deprivation from mo 4–7. G: a previously normal adult cat monocularly deprived for 3 mo

Data From Hubel and Wiesel 113,115 and Blakemore 15


Figure 14.

Critical period for reverse suture. A: ocular dominance histograms in animals with reverse suture following earlier eyelid closure. 1, dominance distribution for control animals monocularly deprived in the right eye until 5 wk. 1–6, results from 5 kittens monocularly deprived in the right (ipsilateral) eye until 5 (2), 6 (3), 8 (4), 10 (5), and 14 (6) wks, respectively, and then reverse sutured. In each case the kitten was then allowed 9 wk of vision using its right (ipsilateral) eye following reverse suturing (left eyelids closed) before recording at the age indicated in parentheses. Numbers on abscissa refer to ocular dominance grouping (see Fig. 12). B: time course of the reversibility of the effects of previous eyelid closure. Reversal index is the ratio of the neurons dominated by the more recently experienced eye (7, 6, and 5) to the total number of visually responsive cells. This index provides an indication of the degree to which the initially deprived eye recaptures the cortex. Broken horizontal line shows the same ratio calculated for the control animals that were monocularly deprived until 5 wk and which had no neurons in groups 7, 6, and 5 (index = 0.0).

From Blakemore & Van Sluyters 19


Figure 15.

Habituation and dishabituation in the acute decerebratespinal preparation. Continuous records of isometric myogram of tibialis anterior under low (<50 g) initial tension. Test stimuli were brief trains (0.5‐s duration) of high frequency (50/s) pulses applied to the skin of the rump at 2‐min intervals. A: 1, habituation and spontaneous recovery. Control rate 1/2 min. Period between arrows indicates response decrement following increase in stimulus frequency to 10/s. Recovery follows after 6 stimuli at the control rate of 1/2 min. 2, restoration of the previously decremented flexion reflex (dishabituation) by introduction of a strong stimulus. Test stimuli were similar to those in 1. Response restoration (during period indicated by shaded rectangle) was produced by a brief pinch to digits of same leg. B: synaptic concomitants of habituation in acute spinal animal. Intracellular recordings of polysynaptic excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) decrement from deep peroneal motor neuron (identified by antidromic activation) in response to single shock stimuli delivered to the superficial peroneal nerve. B: 1, during the control period stimulating rate was 2/min; 2, EPSP decrement during increased stimulation rate at 1‐s intervals; 3, restoration of EPSP amplitude following an extra stimulus delivered to the tibial nerve (tetanus at 100/s for 4‐s duration); 4, subsequent EPSP decrement produced by continued stimulation of 1‐s intervals; 5, spontaneous recovery of PSP amplitude when stimulus rate was restored to 1/2 min. Time and voltage calibration from 1–5, 10 ms and 2 mV.

A, from Spencer et al. 228; B, from Spencer et al. 226,227


Figure 16.

Responses of two types of interneurons in the cat spinal cord during habituation and sensitization. A: H interneuron. Upper graph represents the amplitude of the flexor twitch of tibial anterior muscle showing slight increase (sensitization) followed by habituation and spontaneous recovery (following rest). Lower graph represents mean number of spikes per stimulus of a simultaneously recorded interneuron. H interneuron shows only a progressive decrease in the evoked discharge even during behavioral sensitization. B: S interneuron. Upper and lower graphs again illustrate flexor twitch of the tibial anterior muscle showing sensitization followed by habituation and spontaneous recovery. Lower graph represents mean number of spikes per stimulus of the simultaneously recorded interneuron. S interneuron shows an initial increase followed by a decrease in evoked discharge. C: schematic diagram of possible neuronal substrate of habituation and sensitization. N, nonplastic synapses; H, habituating synapses; S, sensitizing synapses. According to this scheme, afferent cutaneous stimuli exert their influence on two systems: a reflex system (S‐R) that mediates habituation and a “state” system that mediates sensitization.

A and C, from Groves & Thompson 96; B, from Groves et al. 95


Figure 17.

Short‐term habituation and dishabituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. A: 1, defensive withdrawal reflex of siphon and gill. Dorsal view of an intact Aplysia. The parapodia and mantle shelf have been retracted to reveal the gill. The most sensitive area of the receptive field for eliciting the withdrawal reflex consists of two parts: the rostral edge of the mantle shelf containing the purple gland (dark area) and the caudal edge of the mantle shelf and its continuation as the siphon. The surrounding area is less sensitive. The position of the mantle organs at rest (dotted line) is compared to that during withdrawal reflex following tactile stimulation of the siphon (solid lines). 2, sample photocell records illustrating habituation, spontaneous recovery, and dishabituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex. The interval between stimuli (ISI) and the total number of habituatory stimuli are indicated. a, decrement of the response with repetition of the stimulus at 3‐min intervals. Following a 122‐min rest, the response was almost fully recovered. b, later experiment from the same preparation. After rehabituation of the response at 1/min, a dishabituatory stimulus consisting of a strong and prolonged tactile stimulus to the neck region was presented at the arrow. Successive responses were facilitated for several minutes. B: central synaptic changes accompanying habituation, recovery, and dishabituation. 1, preparation used to correlate contraction of the gill and responses of motor neuron L7 is similar to that illustrated in A, except that a slit has been made in the neck in order to externalize the abdominal ganglion. To permit intracellular recordings from individual cells, the ganglion, with its nerves intact, has been pinned to a stage. This is illuminated by a light guide. 2–4, the photocell records of the gill contractions are illustrated on the top traces of each line, and the simultaneous intracellular recordings from an identified motor neuron L7 are illustrated on the bottom traces. Sample records are all from the same preparation. Tactile stimuli (500 ms in duration) were presented to the mantle shelf every 90 s. 2, habituation. Stimuli were presented over a period of 21 min. Number of spikes in the 1‐s interval following the first evoked spike in each trace: 9, 6, 6, 4. 3, partial recovery (after a 9‐min rest) and subsequent rehabituation of the reflex. Number of spikes: 7, 6, 5, 3. 4, dishabituation. Following the last habituation trial shown in the first trace, a strong stimulus was applied to the siphon. The discharge of the motor neuron and the amplitude of the gill contraction progressively increased during the first 3 stimuli following the dishabituatory stimulus and remained elevated for several minutes. Number of spikes: 4, 5, 7, 5. C: changes in the elementary monosynaptic excitatory connections between sensory and motor neurons as a result of repetitive stimulation at an interstimulus interval of 1/10 s. 1, experimental setup used to examine elementary connections in the isolated ganglion. Ganglion was bathed in high Mg2+ (160 mM) and Ca2+ (50 mM) solutions. Sens. N, sensory neuron; M.N., motor neuron. 2–4, Intracellular recording from motor neuron L7 (top trace) and sensory neuron (bottom trace). 2, response decrement of elementary excitatory postsynaptic potential in L7 to 15 repeated stimuli of sensory neuron. Only responses to stimulus numbers 1, 2, 6, 10, and 15 are illustrated. 3, a strong stimulus (6/s train for 4 s) was applied to the connective. This stimulus did not alter the firing pattern of the sensory neuron. 4, following the heterosynaptic stimulus to the connective the amplitude of the elementary EPSP was partially restored.

A, from Pinsker et al. 192, copyright 1970 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; B, from Kupfermann et al. 149, copyright 1970 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; C, V. Castellucci and E. R. Kandel, unpublished data


Figure 18.

Estimation of quantal size and quantal content during depression and facilitation of the monosynaptic connection between sensory and motor cells mediating the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Two independent techniques are used: amplitude histogram and failure analysis. A: Amplitude histograms. 200–300 consecutive synaptic responses were obtained by intracellular stimulation of the sensory neuron at 10‐s intervals. The responses were separated into 3 successive plateau regions, each consisting of 30–100 responses in regions of stability in which the EPSP amplitude changed less than 15%. The histograms reveal a peak of failures followed by a multimodal distribution with the mean of each subsequent peak being an integral multiple of the first unit peak. The unit peak was assumed to represent the quantal unit (q). The dotted line and the arrow on the ordinate indicate the theoretical distribution predicted by the Poisson equation. 1, depression. A comparison of the successive regions illustrates that with repeated stimulation the position of the unit peak (I) does not change, but the relative incidence of failures increases 6‐ to 7‐fold from the first to the third region. Thus the 30 stimuli in the first region produced 2 failures, the next 70 stimuli produced 12 failures (or 6 failures for every 35 responses), and the 35 stimuli in the last region produced 14 failures. These findings indicate that the EPSP depression during continued stimulation is due to a decrease in quantal output (in this case m decreased from 4 to 1) while quantal size remains relatively unaffected. 2, facilitation. Synaptic decrement was first produced with 200 consecutive stimuli to the sensory neuron as in 1. The pathway from the head was then stimulated to produce facilitation. The histograms illustrate the last region of depression (prefacilitation region) just before the facilitating stimulus and the 2 regions following the facilitating stimulus (postfacilitation region). In the last region of depression, there are again a large number of failures. Following the facilitating stimulus, the relative number of failures is markedly reduced and changes from 12 failures or 7/33 responses in the first region to 1/33 responses in the next region. But the position of the unit peak again remains the same. Thus, during facilitation, quantal output increases while the size of a quantal unit does not change. B: Failure analysis. Summary of the results on synaptic depression and synaptic facilitation derived by a failure analysis derived from the Poisson equation whereby the quantal content m0 = lnN/n0 where m0 is the quantal content, N is the number of trials and n0 the number of failures. A first group of 5 experiments was done on synaptic depression and was normalized to the first region prior to facilitation. With both depression and facilitation, the estimated values of quantal size remained constant during successive regions while E, the average excitatory postsynaptic potential amplitude, and m, the quantal content, decreased by 50% during the depression and increased by 100% in relation to the control region during facilitation.



Figure 19.

Excitatory postsynaptic potential decrement accompanying habituation in the crayfish. A: lateral giant dendrite responses in the fifth ganglion to repetitive stimulation of the second root of the same segment at 10/s. The 1st, 2nd, 5th, 10th, and 30th responses are shown. The initial downward deflection is the stimulus artifact. The first fast depolarizing potential is an electrotonic component; the slower later wave is the chemically mediated postsynaptic potentials. B: properties of monosynaptic afferent excitatory postsynaptic potentials in tactile interneuron. 1–3, 1st, 4th, and 7th responses to stimulation at 1/5 s. The small late depolarizations are due to occasional activation of tactile afferents whose spikes are not discernible in the root monitor.

From Zucker 274


Figure 20.

Acquisition and retention of long‐term habituation of defensive withdrawal in Aplysia. A: behavioral experiments. Siphon withdrawal is expressed as a percentage of the median of each group's initial response (block 1, trial 1). The median duration of the initial response was 17 s for the experimental and 19 s for the control animals. For statistical analysis, siphon withdrawal for each animal was expressed as a single score: the sum of 10 trials, that is, the total time an animal spent responding in the 10‐trials habituation session. Intergroup statistical comparisons were made with Mann‐Whitney U tests and intragroup comparisons with Wilcoxon matched‐pairs signed‐ranks tests. Following blocks of siphon‐habituation training spaced by 90 min (acquisition), experimental animals (•) exhibited significantly greater habituation (retention) than control animals (○) at 24 h (P < 0.001). B: acquisition and retention of excitatory postsynaptic potential decrement. The EPSP amplitudes from both experimental (•) and control (○) nerves (n = 10) are expressed as a percentage of the initial amplitude. In acquisition, 6 experiments were run with the siphon nerve as experimental. In block 1, 10 stimuli were first applied to the experimental nerve and then to the control nerve, producing in L7 comparable EPSP decrement from both nerves, which indicated lack of EPSP generalization. Repeated blocks of stimuli to the experimental nerve produced progressive buildup of EPSP decrement. A single test to the control nerve produced an EPSP which was recovered to 84.5% control, indicating that deterioration cannot account for experimental EPSP decrement. In retention, the cell was reimpaled 24 h later and repolarized to the membrane potential maintained for acquisition. The ordinate in retention was redrawn to indicate that the repolarization is only closely approximated, it is not exact. In the retention test, stimulation of the experimental nerve produced significantly greater EPSP decrement (P < 0.001) than stimulation of the control nerve. I:b.i., interblock interval; i.t.i., intertrial interval.



Figure 21.

Spinal cord conditioning. Mean amplitudes of the response to the conditioned stimulus (CS) alone from 20 animals for test trials in acquisition and during five trial blocks in extinction, given as a percent of mean CS alone.

From Patterson et al. 187
References
 1. Adey, W. R. Electrophysiological patterns and cerebral impedance characteristics in orienting and discriminative behavior. Proc. Intern. Congr. Physiol. Sci., 23rd, Tokyo, p. 324–339.
 2. Agranoff, B. W. Agents that block memory. In: The Neurosciences, edited by G. C. Quarton, T. Melnechuk, and F. O. Schmitt. New York: Rockefeller Univ. Press, 1967, p. 756–764.
 3. Agranoff, B. W., R. E. Davis, and J. J. Brink. Chemical studies on memory fixation in goldfish. Brain Res. 1: 303–309, 1966.
 4. Agranoff, B. W., R. E. Davis, L. Casola, and R. Lim. Actinomycin D blocks formation of memory of shock‐avoidance in goldfish. Science 158: 1600–1601, 1967.
 5. Alving, B. O. Spontaneous activity in isolated somata of Aplysia pacemaker neurons. J. Gen. Physiol. 51: 29–45, 1968.
 6. Attardi, D. G., and R. W. Sperry. Preferential selection of central pathways by regenerating optic fibers. Exptl. Neurol. 7: 46–64, 1963.
 7. Atwood, H. L., and G. D. Bittner. Matching of excitatory and inhibitory inputs in crustacean muscle fibers. J. Neurophysiol. 34: 157–170, 1971.
 8. Bailey, C. H., V. F. Castellucci, J. D. Koester, and E. R. Kandel. Central mechanoreceptor neurons in Aplysia connect to peripheral siphon motor neurons: a simple system for the morphological study of the synaptic mechanism underlying habituation. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 1: 588, 1975.
 9. Bartlett, F., and E. R. John. Equipotentiality quantified: the anatomical distribution of the engram. Science 181: 764–767, 1973.
 10. Barondes, S. H., and H. D. Cohen. Puromycin effect on successive phases of memory storage. Science 151: 594–595, 1966.
 11. Barondes, S. H., and H. D. Cohen. Delayed and sustained effect of acetoxycycloheximide on memory in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 58: 157–164, 1967.
 12. Bennett, M. V. L. Comparison of electrically and chemically mediated synaptic transmission. In: Structure and Function of Synapses, edited by G. D. Pappas and D. P. Purpura. New York: Raven, 1972, p. 221–256.
 13. Bennett, M. V. L. Function of electronic junctions in embryonic and adult tissues. Federation Proc. 32: 65–75, 1973.
 14. Beránek, R., P. Hník, L. Vyklický, and J. Zelená. Facilitation of the monosynaptic reflex due to chronic tenotomy. Physiol. Bohemoslov. 10: 543–552, 1961.
 15. Beswick, F. G., and R. T. W. L. Conroy. Optimal tetanic conditioning of heteronymous monosynaptic reflexes. J. Physiol. London 180: 134–146, 1965.
 16. Blakemore, C. Environmental constraints on development in the visual system. In: Constraints on Learning, edited by R. A. Hinde and J. Stevenson‐Hinde. New York: Academic, 1973, p. 51–73.
 17. Blakemore, C. Developmental factors in the formation of feature extracting neurons. In: The Neurosciences. Third Study Program, edited by F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1974, p. 105–113.
 18. Blakemore, C., and G. F. Cooper. Development of the brain depends on the visual environment. Nature 228: 477–478, 1970.
 19. Blakemore, C., and D. E. Mitchell. Environmental modification of the visual cortex and the neural basis of learning and memory. Nature 241: 467–468, 1973.
 20. Blakemore, C., and R. C. Van Sluyters. Reversal of the physiological effects of monocular deprivation in kittens: further evidence for a sensitive period. J. Physiol. London 237: 195–216, 1974.
 21. Blakemore, C., and R. C. Van Sluyters. Innate and environmental factors in the development of the kitten's visual cortex. J. Physiol. London 248: 663–716, 1975.
 22. Bliss, T. V. P., and A. R. Gardner‐Medwin. Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J. Physiol. London 232: 357–374, 1973.
 23. Bliss, T. V. P., and T. Lømo. Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J. Physiol. London 232: 331–356, 1973.
 24. Bradley, K., D. M. Easton, and J. C. Eccles. An investigation of primary or direct inhibition. J. Physiol. London 122: 474–488, 1953.
 25. Brunelli, M., V. Castellucci, and E. R. Kandel. Serotonin mediated increase in cyclic AMP: a possible biochemical step in the increased transmitter output accompanying behavioral sensitization in Aplysia. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 1: 568, 1975.
 26. Bruner, J., and J. Kehoe. Long‐term decrements in the efficacy of synaptic transmission in molluscs and crustaceans. In: Short‐Term Changes in Neural Activity and Behaviour, edited by G. Horn and R. A. Hinde. Cambridge, Engl.: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1970, p. 323–359.
 27. Bruner, J., and D. Kennedy. Habituation: occurrence at a neuromuscular junction. Science 169: 92–94, 1970.
 28. Bruner, J., and L. Tauc. Habituation at the synaptic level in Aplysia. Nature 210: 37–39, 1966.
 29. Buchwald, J. S., and S. G. Schramm. A study of conditioning in chronically spinalized kitten. The Physiologist 8: 125, 1965.
 30. Buerger, A. A., and A. M. Dawson. Spinal kittens: long‐term increases in electromyograms due to a conditioning routine. Physiol. Behavior 3: 99–103, 1968.
 31. Buerger, A. A., and A. M. Dawson. Spinal kittens: effect of clamping of the thoracic aorta on long‐term increases in electromyograms due to a conditioning routine. Exptl. Neurol. 23: 457–464, 1969.
 32. Byrne, J., V. Castellucci, and E. R. Kandel. Receptive fields and response properties of mechanoreceptor neurons innervating siphon skin and mantle shelf in Aplysia. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 1041–1064, 1974.
 33. Cajal, S. Ramón y. La fine structure des centres nerveux. Proc. Roy. Soc. London Ser. B 55: 444–468, 1894.
 34. Cajal, S. Ramón y. Histologie du Système Nerveux de L'homme et des Vertebres. Paris: Maloine, vol. 2, 1911.
 35. Callec, J. J., J. C. Guillet, Y. Pichon, and J. Boistel. Further studies on synaptic transmission in insects. II. Relations between sensory information and its synaptic integration at the level of a single giant axon in the cockroach. J. Exptl. Biol. 55: 123–149, 1971.
 36. Carew, T., V. Castellucci, J. Byrne, and E. Kandel. Quantitative analysis of the contribution of central and peripheral nervous systems to the gill withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 2: 317, 1976.
 37. Carew, T. J., V. F. Castellucci, and E. R. Kandel. An analysis of dishabituation and sensitization of the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Intern. J. Neurosci. 2: 79–98, 1971.
 38. Carew, T. J., and E. R. Kandel. Acquisition and retention of long‐term habituation in Aplysia: correlation of behavioral and cellular processes. Science 182: 1158–1160, 1973.
 39. Carew, T. J., H. M. Pinsker, and E. R. Kandel. Long‐term habituation of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 175: 451–454, 1972.
 40. Carew, T. J., H. Pinsker, K. Rubinson, and E. R. Kandel. Physiological and biochemical properties of neuromuscular transmission between identified motorneurons and gill muscle in Aplysia californica. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 1020–1040, 1974.
 41. Castellucci, V., and E. R. Kandel. A quantal analysis of the synaptic depression underlying habituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. US 71: 5004–5008, 1974.
 42. Castellucci, V., and E. R. Kandel. Quantal analysis of heterosynaptic facilitation underlying dishabituation in Aplysia (Abstract). Federation Proc. 34: 418, 1975.
 43. Castellucci, V., H. Pinsker, H. I. Kupfermann, and E. R. Kandel. Neuronal mechanisms of habituation and dishabituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 167: 1745–1748, 1970.
 44. Cedar, H., E. R. Kandel, and J. H. Schwartz. Cyclic adenosine monophosphate in the nervous system of Aplysia californica. I. Increased synthesis in response to synaptic stimulation. J. Gen. Physiol. 60: 558–569, 1972.
 45. Cedar, H., and J. H. Schwartz. Cyclic adenosine monophosphate in the nervous system of Aplysia californica. II. Effects of serotonin and dopamine. J. Gen. Physiol. 60: 570–587, 1972.
 46. Chow, K. L., and D. L. Stewart. Reversal of structural and functional effects of long‐term visual deprivation in cats. Exptl. Neurol. 34: 409–433, 1972.
 47. Cohen, D. H. Development of a vertebrate experimental model for cellular neurophysiologic studies of learning. Cond. Reflex 4: 61–80, 1969.
 48. Cohen, H. D., and S. H. Barondes. Puromycin effect on memory may be due to occult seizures. Science 157: 333–334, 1967.
 49. Cohen, H. D., F. Ervin, and S. H. Barondes. Puromycin and cycloheximide: different effects on hippocampal electrical activity. Science 154: 1557–1558, 1966.
 50. Cohen, D. H., and R. L. MacDonald. Some variables affecting orienting and conditioned heart‐rate responses in the pigeon. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 74: 123–133, 1971.
 51. Colombo, F., and S. D. Erulkar. Miniature synaptic potentials at frog spinal neurones in the presence of tetrodotoxin. J. Physiol. London 199: 105–221, 1968.
 52. Cragg, B. G. The density of synapses and neurones in the motor and visual areas of the cerebral cortex. J. Anat. 101: 639–654, 1967.
 53. Culler, E. Observations on the spinal dog. Psychol. Bull. 34: 742–743, 1937.
 54. Cyander, M., N. Berman, and A. Hein. Cats reared in stroboscopic illumination: effect on receptive fields in visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. US 70: 1353–1354, 1973.
 55. Dagan, D., and I. Parnas. Giant fibre and small fibre pathways involved in the evasive response of the cockroach. Periplaneta americana. J. Exptl. Biol. 52: 313–324, 1970.
 56. Dews, P. B., and T. N. Wiesel. Consequences of monocular deprivation on visual behaviour in kittens. J. Physiol. London 206: 437–455, 1970.
 57. Dudel, J. Facilitatory effects of 5‐hydroxy‐tryptamine on the crayfish, neuromuscular [Astacus fluviatilis, Orconectes virilis] junction. Arch. Exptl. Pathol. Pharmakol. 249: 515–528, 1965.
 58. Dudel, J., and S. W. Kuffler. Mechanism of facilitation at the crayfish neuromuscular junction. J. Physiol. London 155: 530–542, 1961.
 59. Dudel, J., and S. W. Kuffler. Presynaptic inhibition at the crayfish neuromuscular junction. J. Physiol. London 155: 543–562, 1961.
 60. Duncan, C. P. The retroactive effect of electroshock on learning. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 42: 32–44, 1949.
 61. Dykman, R. A., and P. S. Shurrager. Successive and maintained conditioning in spinal carnivores. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 49: 27–35, 1956.
 62. Eccles, J. C. The Neurophysiological Basis of Mind. The Principles of Neurophysiology. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press, 1953.
 63. Eccles, J. C. The Physiology of Nerve Cells. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press, 1957.
 64. Eccles, J. C. The Physiology of Synapses. Berlin: Springer Verlag, 1964.
 65. Eccles, J. C., and W. J. O'Connor. Abortive impulses at the neuromuscular junction. J. Physiol. London 100: 318–328, 1941.
 66. Eccles, J. C., R. F. Schmidt, and W. D. Willis. Presynaptic inhibition of the spinal monosynaptic reflex pathway. J. Physiol. London 161: 282–297, 1962.
 67. Engel, J., Jr., and C. D. Woody. Effects of character and significance of stimulus on unit activity at coronal‐pericruciate cortex of cat during performance of conditioned motor response. J. Neurophysiol. 35: 220–229, 1972.
 68. Epstein, R., and L. Tauc. Heterosynaptic facilitation and posttetanic potentiation in Aplysia nervous system. J. Physiol. London 209: 1–23, 1970.
 69. Evarts, E. V., and J. R. Hughes. Relation of posttetanic potentiation to subnormality of lateral geniculate potentials. Am. J. Physiol. 188: 238–244, 1957.
 70. Fadiga, E., and J. M. Brookhart. Monosynaptic activation of different portions of the motor neuron membrane. Am. J. Physiol. 198: 693–703, 1960.
 71. Farel, P. B. Long‐lasting habituation in spinal frogs. Brain Res. 33: 405–417, 1971.
 72. Farel, P. B. Persistent increases in synaptic efficacy following brief tetanic stimulation in the isolated frog spinal cord (Abstract). Soc. Neurosci., 3rd Ann. Meeting, 1973. San Diego, p. 41–42.
 73. Farel, P. B., and A. A. Buerger. Instrumental conditioning of leg position in chronic spinal frog; before and after sciatic section. Brain Res. 47: 345, 1972.
 74. Farel, P. B., D. L. Glanzman, and R. F. Thompson. Habituation of a monosynaptic response in vertebrate central nervous system: lateral column‐motoneuron pathway in isolated frog spinal cord. J. Neurophysiol. 36: 1117–1130, 1973.
 75. Feng, T. P. Studies on the neuromuscular junction. XXVI. The changes of the end‐plate potential during and after prolonged stimulation. Chinese J. Physiol. 16: 341–372, 1941.
 76. Fentress, J. C., and R. W. Doty. Protracted tetanization of the optic tract in squirrel monkeys. Federation Proc. 25: 573, 1966.
 77. Fentress, J. C., and R. W. Doty. Effect of tentanization and enucleation upon excitability of visual pathways in squirrel monkeys and cats. Exptl. Neurol. 30 (3): 535–554, 1971.
 78. Fifkova, E. The effect of monocular deprivation on the synaptic contacts of the visual cortex. J. Neurobiol. 1: 285–294, 1970.
 79. Fischbach, G. D. Synapse formation between dissociated nerve and muscle cells in low density cell cultures. Develop. Biol. 28: 407–429, 1972.
 80. Fitzgerald, L. A., and R. F. Thompson. Classical conditioning of the hindlimb flexion reflex in the acute spinal cat. Psychonomic Sci. 8: 213–214, 1967.
 81. Flexner, J. B., L. B. Flexner, and E. Stellar. Memory in mice as affected by intracerebral puromycin. Science 141: 57–59, 1963.
 82. Flexner, L. B., and J. B. Flexner. Effect of acetoxycyclo‐heximide and of an acetoxycycloheximide‐puromycin mixture on cerebral protein synthesis and memory in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 55: 369–374, 1966.
 83. Flexner, L. B., J. B. Flexner, and R. B. Roberts. Memory in mice analyzed with antibiotics. Science 155: 1377–1382, 1967.
 84. Forbes, A. The interpretation of spinal reflexes in terms of present knowledge of nerve conduction. Physiol. Rev. 2: 361–414, 1922.
 85. Frank, K., and M. G. F. Fuortes. Presynaptic and postsynaptic inhibition of monosynaptic reflexes. Federation Proc. 16: 39–40, 1957.
 86. Frazier, W. T., E. R. Kandel, I. Kupfermann, R. Waziri, and R. E. Coggeshal. Morphological and functional properties of identified neurons in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia californica. J. Neurophysiol. 30: 1288–1351, 1967.
 87. Frazier, W. T., R. Waziri, and E. R. Kandel. Alterations in the frequency of spontaneous activity in Aplysia neurons with contingent and non‐contingent nerve stimulation. Federation Proc. 24: 522, 1965.
 88. Furshpan, E. J., and D. D. Potter. Low‐resistance junctions between cells in embryos and tissue culture. In: Current Topics in Developmental Biology, edited by A. A. Moscona and A. Monroy. New York: Academic, 1968; vol. 3, p. 95–127.
 89. Garcia, J., B. K. McGowan, and K. F. Green. Biological constraints on conditioning. In: Classical Conditioning. II. Current Research and Theory, edited by A. H. Black and W. F. Prokas. New York: Appleton‐Century‐Crofts, 1972, p. 3–27.
 90. Gaze, R. M. The Formation of Nerve Connections; A Consideration of Neural Specificity Modulation and Comparable Phenomena. New York: Academic, 1970.
 91. Gelperin, A. Rapid food aversion learning by a terrestrial mollusk. Science 189: 567–570, 1975.
 92. Gerard, R. W. Physiology and psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 106: 161–173, 1949.
 93. Getting, P., and A. O. D. Willows. Modification of neuron properties by electronic synapses. II. Burst formation by electrotonic synapses. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 858–868, 1974.
 94. Glassman, E. The biochemistry of learning: an evaluation of the role of RNA and protein. Ann. Rev. Biochem. 38: 605–646, 1969.
 95. Glassman, E. Macromolecules and behavior: a commentary. In: The Neurosciences. Third Study Program, edited by F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1974, p. 667–677.
 96. Goldberg, A. L., and J. J. Singer. Evidence for a role of cyclic AMP in neuromuscular transmission. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. US 64: 134–141, 1969.
 97. Goldfarb, J., and R. U. Muller. Occurrence of heteronymous monosynaptic reflexes following tenotomy. Brain Res. 28: 553–555, 1971.
 98. Göpfert, H., and H. Schaeffer. Über den direkt und indirekt erregten Aktionsstrom und die Funktion der motorischen Endplatte. Arch. Ges. Physiol. 239: 597–619, 1938.
 99. Greengard, P., and J. W. Kebabian. Role of cyclic AMP in synaptic transmission in the mammalian peripheral nervous system. Federation Proc. 33: 1059–1067, 1974.
 100. Groves, P. M., R. DeMarco, and R. F. Thompson. Habituation and sensitization of spinal interneuron activity in acute spinal cat. Brain Res. 14: 521–525, 1969.
 101. Groves, P. M., and R. F. Thompson. Habituation: a dualprocess theory. Psychol. Rev. 77: 419–450, 1970.
 102. Guillery, R. W. Binocular competition in the control of geniculate cell growth. J. Comp. Neurol. 144: 117–130, 1972.
 103. Guillery, R. W. The effect of lid suture upon the growth of cells in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of kittens. J. Comp. Neurol. 148: 417–422, 1973.
 104. Guillery, R. W., and D. J. Stelzner. The differential effects of unilateral lid closure upon the monocular and binocular segments of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 139: 413–422, 1970.
 105. Hagbarth, K.‐E., and B. L. Finer. The plasticity of human withdrawal reflexes to noxious skin stimuli in lower limbs. In: Progress in Brain Research. Brain Mechanisms. Specific and Unspecific Mechanisms of Sensory Motor Integration, edited by G. Moruzzi, A. Fessard, and H. H. Jasper. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1963, vol. 1, p. 65–81.
 106. Hagbarth, K. E., and E. Kugelberg. Plasticity of the human abdominal skin reflex. Brain 81: 305–318, 1958.
 107. Harlow, H. F., and M. K. Harlow. Social deprivation in monkeys. Sci. Am. 207: 136–146, 1962.
 108. Hebb, D. O. The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York: Wiley, 1949.
 109. Heinroth, O. Beiträge zur Biologie, namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Proc. Intern. Ornithol. Congr., 5th, Berlin, 1910, p. 589–702.
 110. Hilgard, E. R., and D. G. Marquis. Conditioning and Learning. New York: Appleton‐Century‐Crofts, 1940.
 111. Hinde, R. A., and J. Stevenson‐Hinde (editors). Constraints on Learning; Limitations and Predispositions. London: Academic, 1973.
 112. Hirsch, H. V. B. Visual perception in cats after environmental surgery. Exptl. Brain Res. 15: 405–423, 1972.
 113. Hirsch, H. V. B., and D. N. Spinelli. Visual experience modifies distribution of horizontally and vertically oriented receptive fields in cats. Science 168: 869–871, 1970.
 114. Hirsch, H. V. B., and D. N. Spinelli. Modification of the distribution of receptive field orientation in cats by selective visual exposure during development. Exptl. Brain Res. 13: 509–527, 1971.
 115. Hník, P., R. Beránek, L. Vyklický, and J. Zelená. Sensory outflow from chronically tenotomized muscles. Physiol. Bohemoslov. 12: 23–29, 1963.
 116. Hubel, D. H. Effects of distortion of sensory imput on the visual system of kittens. Eleventh Bowditch lecture. The Physiologist 10: 17–45, 1967.
 117. Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J. Physiol. London 160: 106–154, 1962.
 118. Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel. Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. J. Neurophysiol. 26: 994–1002, 1963.
 119. Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel. Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with artificial squint. J. Neurophysiol. 28: 1041–1059, 1965.
 120. Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel. The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. J. Physiol. London 206: 419–436, 1970.
 121. Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel. Sequence regularity and geometry of orientation columns in the monkey striate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 158: 267–293, 1974.
 122. Hubel, D. H., T. N. Wiesel, and S. Leray. Functional architecture of area 17 in normal and monocularly deprived macaque monkeys. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. XL: 581–589, 1975.
 123. Hubel, D. H., T. N. Wiesel, and S. Levay. Plasticity of ocular dominance in monkey striate cortex. Phil. Trans. Soc. Lond. B: 133–163, 1976.
 124. Hughes, J. R., E. V. Evarts, and W. H. Marshall. Posttetanic potentiation in the visual system of cats. Am. J. Physiol. 186: 483–487, 1956.
 125. Hutter, O. F. Post‐tetanic restoration of neuromuscular transmission blocked by D‐tubocurarine. J. Physiol. London 118: 216–227, 1952.
 126. Hydén, H. Biochemical changes accompanying learning. In: The Neurosciences. First Study Program, edited by G. C. Quarton, T. Melnechuk, and F. O. Schmitt. New York: Rockefeller Univ. Press, 1967, p. 765–771.
 127. Hydén, H., and P. W. Lange. Do specific biochemical correlates to learning processes exist in brain cells? In: Biochemistry of Simple Neuronal Models. Advances in Biochemical Psychopharmacology, edited by E. Costa and E. Giacobini. New York: Raven, 1970, vol. 2, p. 317–338.
 128. Ito, M. The control mechanisms of cerebellar motor system. In: The Neurosciences. Third Study Program, edited by F. O. Schmitt and F. G. Worden. Boston: MIT Press, 1974, p. 293–303.
 129. Ito, M. Cerebellar learning control of the vestibular‐ocular mechanism. In: Mechanisms in the Transmission of Signals for Conscious Behavior. In press. (Satellite Symp., XXVI, Intern. Physiol. Congress, 1975.).
 130. Ito, M. Learning control mechanisms by the cerebellum in the flocculo‐vestibulo ocular system. In: Nervous System, Vol 1, edited by Donald B. Tower. New York: Raven, 1976, p. 245–252.
 131. Jacklet, J. W., and K. Lukowiak. Neural processes in habituation and sensitization in model systems. In: Progress in Neurobiology, Vol. 4 (part 1), edited by G. A. Kerkut and J. W. Phillis. Oxford: Pergamon, 1974, p. 1–56.
 132. Jacobsen, M. Developmental Neurobiology. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1970.
 133. Jahan‐Parvar, B., and R. v. Baumgarten. Untersuchungen zur Spezifitaetsfrage der heterosynaptischen Facilitation bei Aplysia californica. Arch. Ges. Physiol. 295: 347–360, 1967.
 134. John, E. R. Mechanisms of Memory. New York: Academic, 1967.
 135. John, E. R. Switchboard versus statistical theories of learning and memory. Science 177: 850–864, 1972.
 136. Kandel, E. R. Cellular studies of learning. In: The Neurosciences. First Study Program, edited by G. C. Quarton, T. Melnechuk and F. O. Schmitt. New York: Rockefeller Univ. Press, 1967, p. 666–689.
 137. Kandel, E. R., M. Brunelli, J. Byrne, and V. Castellucci. A common presynaptic locus for the synaptic changes underlying short‐term habituation and sensitization. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. LX: The Synapse, 1976, p. 465–482.
 138. Kandel, E. R., W. T. Frazier, R. Waziri, and R. E. Coggeshall. Direct and common connections among identified neurons in Aplysia. J. Neurophysiol. 30: 1352–1376, 1967.
 139. Kandel, E. R., and W. A. Spencer. Cellular neurophysiological approaches in the study of learning. Physiol. Rev. 48: 65–134, 1968.
 140. Kandel, E. R., and L. Tauc. Heterosynaptic facilitation in neurones of the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia depilans. J. Physiol. London 181: 1–27, 1965.
 141. Kandel, E. R., and L. Tauc. Mechanism of heterosynaptic facilitation in the giant cell of the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia depilans. J. Physiol. London 181: 28–47, 1965.
 142. Kandel, E. R., and L. Tauc. Anomalous rectification in the metacerebral giant cells and its consequences for synaptic transmission. J. Physiol. London 183: 287–304, 1966.
 143. Kater, S. B., and C. H. Fraser Rowell. Integration of sensory and centrally programmed components in generation of cyclical feeding activity of Helisoma trivolvis. J. Neurophysiol. 36: 142–155, 1973.
 144. Katz, B., and R. Miledi. A study of spontaneous miniature potentials in spinal motoneurones. J. Physiol. London 168: 389–422, 1963.
 145. Katz, B., and R. Miledi. The timing of calcium action during neuromuscular transmission. J. Physiol. London 189: 535–544, 1967.
 146. Katz, B., and R. Miledi. The release of acetylcholine from nerve endings by graded electric pulses. Proc. Roy. Soc. London Ser. B 167: 23–38, 1967.
 147. Katz, B., and R. Miledi. The role of calcium in neuromuscular facilitation. J. Physiol. London 195: 481–491, 1968.
 148. Kellogg, W. N., J. Deese, N. H. Pronko, and M. Feinberg. An attempt to condition the chronic spinal dog. J. Exptl. Psychol. 37: 99–117, 1947.
 149. Kennedy, D., R. L. Calebrese, and J. J. Wine. Presynaptic inhibition: primary afferent depolarization in crayfish neurons. Science 186: 451–453, 1974.
 150. Kennedy, D., and K. Takeda. Reflex control of abdominal flexor muscles in the crayfish. II. The tonic system. J. Exptl. Biol. 43: 229–246, 1965.
 151. Kimble, G. A. Hilgard and Marquis' Conditioning and Learning (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton‐Century‐Crofts, 1961.
 152. Kling, J. W. Learning: introductory survey. In: Woodworth and Schlosberg's Experimental Psychology (3rd ed.), edited by J. W. Kling and L. A. Riggs. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1971, p. 551–613.
 153. Konorski, J. Conditioned Reflexes and Neuron Organization. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1948.
 154. Kozak, W., W. V. Macfarlane, and R. Westerman. Longlasting reversible changes in the reflex responses of chronic spinal cats to touch, heat and cold. Nature 193: 171–173, 1962.
 155. Kozak, W., and R. A. Westerman. Plastic changes of spinal monosynaptic responses from tenotomized muscles in cats. Nature 189: 753–755, 1961.
 156. Krasne, F. B. Excitation and habituation of the crayfish escape reflex: the depolarizing response in lateral giant fibres of the isolated abdomen. J. Exptl. Biol. 50: 29–46, 1969.
 157. Krasne, F. B., and J. S. Bryan. Habituation: regulation through presynaptic inhibition. Science 182: 590–592, 1973.
 158. Krasne, F. B., and K. S. Woodsmall. Waning of the crayfish escape response as a result of repeated stimulation. Animal Behav. 17: 416–424, 1969.
 159. Kuba, K. Effects of catecholamines on the neuromuscular junction in the rat diaphragm. J. Physiol. London 211: 551–570, 1970.
 160. Kuno, M. Mechanism of facilitation and depression of the excitatory synaptic potential in spinal motoneurones. J. Physiol. London 175: 100–112, 1964.
 161. Kuno, M., and J. N. Weakly. Quantal components of the inhibitory synaptic potential in spinal motoneurones of the cat. J. Physiol. London 224: 287–303, 1972.
 162. Kupfermann, I., T. J. Carew, and E. R. Kandel. Local, reflex and central commands controlling gill and siphon movements in Aplysia. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 996–1019, 1974.
 163. Kupfermann, I., V. Castellucci, H. Pinsker, and E. R. Kandel. Neuronal correlates of habituation and dishabituation of the gill‐withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 167: 1743–1745, 1970.
 164. Kupfermann, I., and E. R. Kandel. Neuronal controls of a behavioral response mediated by the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia. Science 164: 847–850, 1969.
 165. Landau, E. M., A. Smolinsky, and Y. Lass. Post‐tetanic potentiation and facilitation do not share a common calcium‐dependent mechanism. Nature New Biol. 244: 155–157, 1973.
 166. Larimer, J. L., A. C. Eggleston, L. M. Masukawa, and D. Kennedy. The different connections and motor outputs of lateral and medial giant fibres in the crayfish. J. Exptl. Biol. 54: 391–402, 1971.
 167. Larrabee, M. G., and D. W. Bronk. Prolonged facilitation of synaptic excitation in sympathetic ganglia. J. Neurophysiol. 10: 139–154, 1947.
 168. Larrabee, M. G., J. D. Klingman, and W. S. Leicht. Effects of temperature, calcium and activity on phospholipid metabolism in a sympathetic ganglion. J. Neurochem. 10: 549–570, 1963.
 169. Larrabee, M. G., and W. S. Leicht. Metabolism of phosphatidyl inositol and other lipids in active neurones of sympathetic ganglia and other peripheral nervous tissues. The site of the inositide effect. J. Neurochem. 12: 1–13, 1965.
 170. Lashley, K. S. Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence: a Quantitative Study of Injuries of the Brain. Chicago: Chicago Univ. Press, 1929.
 171. Lehninger, A. L. Mitochondria and calcium ion transport. Biochem. J. 119: 129–138, 1970.
 172. Levitan, I. B., and S. H. Barondes. Octopamine and serotonin‐stimulated phosphorylation of specific protein in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia californica. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. US 71: 1145–1148, 1974.
 173. Liley, A. W. An investigation of spontaneous activity at the neuromuscular junction of the rat. J. Physiol. London 132: 650–666, 1956.
 174. Lloyd, D. P. C. Post‐tetanic potentiation of response in monosynaptic reflex pathways of the spinal cord. J. Gen. Physiol. 33: 147–170, 1949.
 175. Lloyd, D. P. C., and V. J. Wilson. Reflex depression in rhythmically active monosynaptic reflex pathways. J. Gen. Physiol. 40: 409–426, 1957.
 176. Lorente de Nó, R. Analysis of the activity of the chains of internuncial neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1: 207–244, 1938.
 177. Lorenz, K. Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes. Naturwissenschaften 25: 289–300; 307–308; 324–331;, 1937. [Translated into English by R. Martin as The Establishment of the Instinct Concept. In: Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, edited by K. Lorenz. London: Methuen, 1970, vol. 1.].
 178. Lorenz, K. Z. The comparative method in studying innate behaviour patterns. Symp. Soc. Exptl. Biol. 4: 221–268, 1950.
 179. Lugaro, E. I recenti progressi dell' anatomia del sistema nervoso in rapporto alla psicologia ed alla psichiatria. Riv. Sper. Freniatria 26: 831–894, 1900. [Cited in Cajal .].
 180. MacKay, A. R., and A. Gelperin. Pharmacology and reflex responsiveness of the heart in the giant garden slug, Limax maximus. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. (A) 43: 877–896, 1972.
 181. Magleby, K. L. The effect of tetanic and post‐tetanic potentiation on facilitation of transmitter release at the frog neuromuscular junction. J. Physiol. London 234: 353–371, 1973.
 182. Marmor, M. F. The effects of temperature and ions on the current‐voltage relation and electrical characteristics of a molluscan neurone. J. Physiol. London 218: 573–598, 1971.
 183. Mark, R. F. Chemospecific synaptic repression as a possible memory store. Nature 225: 178–179, 1970.
 184. Mark, R. F., L. R. Marotte, and J. R. Johnstone. Reinnervated eye muscles do not respond to impulses in foreign nerves. Science 170: 193–194, 1970.
 185. Martin, A. R., and G. Pilar. Dual mode of synaptic transmission in the avian ciliary ganglion. J. Physiol. London 168: 443–463, 1963.
 186. McAfee, D. A., M. Schorderet, and P. Greengard. Adenosine 3′, 5′‐monophosphate in nervous tissue: increase associated with synaptic transmission. Science 171: 1156–1158, 1971.
 187. McCleary, R. A. Response specificity in the behavioral effects of limbic system lesions of the cat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 54: 605–613, 1961.
 188. Miller, N. E. Certain facts of learning relevant to the search for its physical basis. In: The Neurosciences. First Study Program, edited by G. C. Quarton, T. Melnechuk, and F. O. Schmitt. New York: Rockefeller Univ. Press, 1967, p. 643–652.
 189. Mize, R., and E. W. Murphy. Selective visual experience fails to modify receptive field properties of rabbit striate cortex neurons. Science 180: 320–323, 1973.
 190. Movshon, J. A. Reversal of the behavioral effects of monocular deprivation in the kitten. J. Physiol 261: 175–187, 1976.
 191. Mpitsos, G. J., and S. D. Collins. Learning: rapid aversive conditioning in the gastropod mollusc Pleurobranchaca. Science 188: 954–957, 1975.
 192. Mpitsos, G. J., and W. J. Davis. Leaning: classical and avoidance conditioning in the mollusk Pleurobranchaea. Science 180: 317–320, 1973.
 193. Muir, D. W., and D. E. Mitchell. Visual resolution and experience: acuity deficits in cats following early selective visual deprivation. Science 180: 420–422, 1973.
 194. Myers, R. E. Corpus callosum and visual gnosis. In: Brain Mechanisms and Learning, edited by J. F. Delafresnaye, A. Fessard, R. W. Gerard, and J. Konorski. Oxford: Blackwell, 1961, p. 481–505.
 195. Myers, R. E., and R. W. Sperry. Interhemispheric communication through the corpus callosum: mnemonic carry‐over between the hemispheres. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat. 80: 298–303, 1958.
 196. Nesmeianova, T. N. The inhibition of the motor reflex in spinal dogs under conditions of chronic experimentation. [English Translation.] Sechenov Physiol. J. USSR 43: 281–288, 1957.
 197. Nicholls, J. G., and D. A. Baylor. Specific modalities and receptive fields of sensory neurons in CNS of the leech. J. Neurophysiol. 31: 740–756, 1968.
 198. Nicholls, J. G., and D. Purves. Monosynaptic chemical and electrical connexions between sensory and motor cells in the central nervous system of the leech. J. Physiol. London 209: 647–667, 1970.
 199. Niklaus, R. Die Erregung einzelner Fadenhaare von Periplaneta americana in Abhängigkeit von der Grösse und Richtung der Auslenkung. Z. Vgl. Physiol. 50: 331–362, 1965.
 200. Olds, J., J. F. Disterhoft, M. Segal, C. L. Kornblith, and R. Hirsch. Learning centers of the rat brain mapped by measuring latencies of conditioned unit responses. J. Neurophysiol. 35: 202–219, 1972.
 201. Parnas, I., D. Armstrong, and F. Strumwasser. Prolonged excitatory and inhibitory synaptic modulation of a bursting pacemaker neuron. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 594–608, 1974.
 202. Parnas, I., and F. Strumwasser. Mechanisms of long lasting inhibition of a bursting pacemaker neuron. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 609–620, 1974.
 203. Patrick, R. L., and N. Kirshner. Effect of stimulation on the levels of tyrosine hydroxylase, dopamine β‐hydroxylase, and catecholamines in intact and denervated rat adrenal glands. Mol. Pharmacol. 7: 87–96, 1971.
 204. Patterson, M. M., C. F. Cegavske, and R. F. Thompson. Effects of a classical conditioning paradigm on hind‐limb flexor nerve response in immobilized spinal cats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 84: 88–97, 1973.
 205. Pavlov, I. P. Conditioned Reflexes; an Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. London: Oxford Univ. Press, 1927.
 206. Pettigrew, J. D. The effect of visual experience on the development of stimulus specificity by kitten cortical neurones. J. Physiol. London 237: 49–74, 1974.
 207. Pettigrew, J. D., and R. D. Freeman. Visual experience without lines: effect on developing cortical neurons. Science 182: 599–600, 1973.
 208. Pettigrew, J., C. Olson, and H. B. Barlow. Kitten visual cortex: short‐term, stimulus‐induced changes in connectivity. Science 180: 1202–1203, 1973.
 209. Pinsker, H., and E. Kandel. Contingent modification of an endogenous bursting rhythm by monosynaptic inhibition (Abstract). The Physiologist 10: 279, 1967.
 210. Pinsker, H., I. Kupfermann, V. Castellucci, and E. Kandel. Habituation and dishabituation of the gill‐with‐drawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 167: 1740–1742, 1970.
 211. Pinsker, H. M., W. A. Hening, T. J. Carew, and E. R. Kandel. Long‐term sensitization of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science 182: 1039–1042, 1973.
 212. Pribram, K. H. Languages of the Brain; Experimental Paradoxes and Principles in Neuropsychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, 1971.
 213. Prosser, C. L., and W. S. Hunter. The extinction of startle responses and spinal reflexes in the white rat. Am. J. Physiol. 117: 609–618, 1936.
 214. Pumphrey, R. J., and A. F. Rawdon‐Smith. Synaptic transmission of nervous impulses through the last abdominal ganglion of the cockroach. Proc. Roy. Soc. London Ser. B 122: 106–118, 1937.
 215. Rahaminoff, R. A dual effect of calcium ions on neuromuscular facilitation. J. Physiol. London 184: 473–498, 1968.
 216. Rahaminoff, R., and E. Alnaes. Inhibitory action of ruthenium red on neuromuscular transmission. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. US 70: 3613–3616, 1973.
 217. Raisman, G. Neuronal plasticity in the septal nuclei of the adult rat. Brain Res. 14: 25–48, 1969.
 218. Rall, T. W., and A. G. Gilman. The role of cyclic AMP in the nervous system. Neurosci. Res. Program. Bull. 8: 221–323, 1970.
 219. Rasmussen, H. Cell communication, calcium ion and cyclic adenosine monophosphate. Science 170: 404–412, 1970.
 220. Rensch, B., and L. Franzisket. Lang andauernde bedingte Reflexe bei Rückenmarksfröschen. Z. Vgl. Physiol. 36: 318–326, 1954.
 221. Riesen, A. H. Plasticity of behavior: psychological series. In: Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior, H. F. Harlow and C. N. Woolsey. Wisconsin: Univ. of Wisconsin Press, 1958, p. 425–450.
 222. Robbins, N., and G. D. Fischbach. Effects of chronic disuse of rat soleus neuromuscular junction on presynaptic function. J. Neurophysiol. 34: 570–578, 1970.
 223. Robbins, N., and P. G. Nelson. Tenotomy and the spinal monosynaptic reflex. Exptl. Neurol. 27: 66–75, 1970.
 224. Robison, G. A., R. W. Butcher, and E. W. Sutherland. Adenyl cyclase as an adrenergic receptor. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 139: 703–723, 1971.
 225. Roeder, K. D. Organization of the ascending giant fiber system in the cockroach (Periplaneta americana). J. Exptl. Zool. 108: 243–261, 1948.
 226. Rosenthal, J. Post‐tetanic potentiation at the neuromuscular junction of the frog. J. Physiol. London 203: 121–133, 1969.
 227. Rudomin, P., R. Nuñez, J. Madrid, and R. E. Burke. Primary afferent hyperpolarization and presynaptic facilitation of Ia afferent terminals induced by large cutaneous fibers. J. Neurophysiol. 37: 413–429, 1974.
 228. Schaeffer, H., and P. Haass. Über einen lokalen Erregungsstrom an der motorischen Endplatte. Arch. Ges. Physiol. 242: 364–381, 1938.
 229. Schleidt, W. M. Über die Auslösung der Flucht vor Raubvögeln bei Truthühnern. Naturwissenschaften 48: 141–142, 1961.
 230. Schleidt, W. M. Reaktionen von Truthühnern auf fliegende Raubvö gel und Versuche zur Analyse ihrer AAM's. Z. Tierpsychol. 18: 534–560, 1961.
 231. Schneider, G. E. Mechanisms of functional recovery following lesions of visual cortex or superior colliculus in neonate and adult hamsters. Brain Behav. Evol. 3: 295–323, 1970.
 232. Schwartz, J. H., V. F. Castellucci, and E. R. Kandel. Functioning of identified neurons and synapses in abdominal ganglion of Aplysia in absence of protein synthesis. J. Neurophysiol. 34: 939–953, 1971.
 233. Segaar, J. Die Funktion des Vorderhirns in Bezug auf des angeborene Berhalten des dreidornigen Stichlingsmännchen (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.) — zugleich ein Beitrag über Neuronenregeneration im Fischgehirn. Acta Morphol. Neerl. Scand. 5: 49–64, 1962.
 234. Seligman, M. E. P., and J. L. Hager. Biological Boundaries of Learning. New York: Appleton‐Century‐Crofts, 1972.
 235. Senden, M. Von. Raum‐und Gestaltauffassung bei operierten Blindgeborenen vor und nach der Operation. Leipzig: Barth, 1932.
 236. Sharpless, S. K., and H. Jasper. Habituation of the arousal reaction. Brain 79: 655–680, 1956.
 237. Sherman, R. G., and H. L. Atwood. Synaptic facilitation: long‐term neuromuscular facilitation in crustaceans. Science 171: 1248–1250, 1971.
 238. Sherrington, C. S. The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. New Haven: Yale Univ. Press, 1906.
 239. Shimahara, T., and L. Tauc. Heterosynaptic facilitation in the giant cell of Aplysia. J. Physiol. London 247: 321–341, 1972.
 240. Shurrager, P. S., and E. A. Culler. Phenomena allied to conditioning in the spinal dog. Am. J. Physiol. 123: 186–187, 1938.
 241. Shurrager, P. S., and E. Culler. Conditioned extinction of a reflex in the spinal dog. J. Exptl. Psychol. 28: 287–303, 1941.
 242. Sokolov, E. N. Higher nervous functions: the orienting reflex. Ann. Rev. Physiol. 25: 545–580, 1963.
 243. Spencer, W. A., and R. S. April. Plastic properties of monosynaptic pathways in mammals. In: Short‐Term Changes in Neural Activity and Behaviour, edited by G. Horn and R. A. Hinde. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1970, p. 433–474.
 244. Spencer, W. A., R. F. Thompson, and D. R. Neilson, Jr. Response decrement of the flexion reflex in the acute spinal cat and transient restoration by strong stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 29: 221–239, 1966.
 245. Spencer, W. A., R. F. Thompson, and D. R. Neilson, Jr. Decrement of ventral root electrotonus and intracellularly recorded PSPs produced by iterated cutaneous afferent volleys. J. Neurophysiol. 29: 253–274, 1966.
 246. Spencer, W. A., and R. Wigdor. Ultra‐late PTP of monosynaptic reflex responses in cat (Abstract). The Physiologist 8: 278, 1965.
 247. Sperry, R. W. Mechanisms of neural maturation. In: Handbook of Experimental Psychology, edited by S. S. Stevens. New York: Wiley, 1951, p. 247–252.
 248. Sperry, R. W. Selective communication in nerve nets: impulse specificity vs. connection specificity. Neurosci. Res. Program Bull. 3 (5): 37–43, 1965.
 249. Spira, M. E., and M. V. L. Bennett. Synaptic control of electrotonic coupling between neurons. Brain Res. 37: 294–300, 1972.
 250. Stent, G. S. A physiological mechanism for Hebb's postulate of learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. US 70: 997–1001, 1973.
 251. Stephens, C. L. Progressive decrements in the activity of Aplysia neurones following repeated intracellular stimulation: implications for habituation. J. Exptl. Biol. 58: 411–421, 1973.
 252. Steward, O., C. W. Cotman, and G. S. Lynch. Re‐establishment of electrophysiologically functional entorhinal cortical input to the dentate gyrus deafferented by ipsilateral entorhinal lesions: innervation by the contralateral entorhinal cortex. Exptl. Brain Res. 18: 396–414, 1973.
 253. Strumwasser, F. Types of information stored in single neurons. In: Invertebrate Nervous Systems. Their Significance for Mammalian Neurophysiology, edited by C. A. G. Wiersma. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press, 1967, p. 291–319.
 254. Stryker, M. P., and H. Sherk. Modification of cortical orientation selectivity in the cat by restricted visual exposure: a reexamination. Science 190: 904–906, 1975.
 255. Tanzi, E. I fatti e le induzioni nell'odierna istologia del sistema nervoso. Riv. Sper. Freniatia 19: 419–472, 1893. [Cited in Cajal .].
 256. Tauc, L. Presynaptic inhibition in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia. J. Physiol. London 181: 282–307, 1965.
 257. Tauc, L. Polyphasic synaptic activity. In: Progress in Brain Research. Mechanisms of Synaptic Transmission, edited by K. Akert and P. G. Waser. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1969. vol. 31, p. 247–257.
 258. Tauc, L., and R. Epstein. Heterosynaptic facilitation as a distinct mechanism in Aplysia. Nature 214: 724–725, 1967.
 259. Thoenen, H. Induction of tyrosine hydroxylase in peripheral and central adrenergic neurones by cold‐exposure of rats. Nature 228: 861–862, 1970.
 260. Thompson, R. Localization of the “visual memory system” in the white rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. Monogr. 69: 45, part 2, 1969.
 261. Thompson, R. F., and W. A. Spencer. Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol. Rev. 73: 16–43, 1966.
 262. Thorpe, W. H. Learning and Instinct in Animals. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press, 1956.
 263. Tinbergen, N. The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Clarendon, 1951.
 264. Valverde, F. Apical dendritic spines of the visual cortex and light deprivation in the mouse. Exptl. Brain Res. 3: 337–352, 1967.
 265. Valverde, F. Structural changes in the area striata of the mouse after enucleation. Exptl. Brain Res. 5: 274–292, 1968.
 266. Von Baumgarten, R. J. Plasticity in the nervous system at the unitary level. In: The Neurosciences, Second Study Program, edited by F. O. Schmitt. New York: Rockefeller Univ. Press, 1970, p. 260–271.
 267. Von Baumgarten, R. J., and B. Djahanparwar. Time course of repetitive heterosynaptic facilitation in Aplysia californica. Brain Res. 4: 295–297, 1967.
 268. Von Baumgarten, R., and B. Jahan‐Parwar. Beitrag zur Problem der heterosynaptischen Facilitation in Aplysia californica. Arch. Ges. Physiol. 295: 328–346, 1967.
 269. Wall, P. D. Habituation and post‐tetanic potentiation in the spinal cord. In: Short‐Term Changes in Neural Activity and Behaviour, edited by G. Horn and R. A. Hinde.Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1970, p. 181–210.
 270. Waziri, R., and E. R. Kandel. Organization of inhibition in abdominal ganglion of Aplysia. III. Interneurons mediating inhibition. J. Neurophysiol. 32: 520–539, 1969.
 271. Weinreich, D. Ionic mechanism of post‐tetanic potentiation at the neuromuscular junction of the frog. J. Physiol. London 212: 431–446, 1971.
 272. Wells, M. J., and J. Wells. Conditioning and sensitization in snails. Animal Behav. 19: 305–312, 1971.
 273. Wickelgren, B. G. Habituation of spinal motoneurons. J. Neurophysiol. 30: 1404–1423, 1967.
 274. Wickelgren, B. G. Habituation of spinal interneurons. J. Neurophysiol. 30: 1424–1438, 1967.
 275. Wiesel, T. N., and D. H. Hubel. Single‐cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J. Neurophysiol. 26: 1003–1017, 1963.
 276. Wiesel, T. N., and D. H. Hubel. Comparison of the effects of unilateral and bilateral eye closure on cortical unit responses in kittens. J. Neurophysiol. 28: 1029–1040, 1965.
 277. Wiesel, T. N., and D. H. Hubel. Ordered arrangement of orientation columns in monkeys lacking visual experience. J. Comp. Neurol. 158: 307–318, 1974.
 278. Wiesel, T. N., and D. H. Hubel. Reorganization of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. (Abstract). Ann. Meeting Soc. Neurosci., 4th, St. Louis, 1974, p. 740.
 279. Willey, T. J., and W. J. Freeman. Alteration of the evoked response from prepyriform cortex following prolonged electrical stimulation of the lateral olfactory tract (Abstract). The Physiologist 12: 394, 1969.
 280. Wine, J. J., and F. B. Krasne. Independence of inhibition and habituation in the crayfish lateral giant fiber escape reflex. In: Proceedings of the Seventy‐Seventh Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C. Washington, D.C.: APA, 1969, p. 237–238.
 281. Wine, J. J., and F. B. Krasne. The organization of escape behaviour in the crayfish. J. Exptl. Biol. 56: 1–18, 1972.
 282. Woodworth, R. S., and H. Schlosberg. Experimental Psychology, New York: Holt, Reinhart, Winston, 1954, p. 786–794.
 283. Woody, C. D. Conditioned eye blink: gross potential activity at coronal‐precruciate cortex of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 33: 838–850, 1970.
 284. Woody, C. D., and P. Black‐Cleworth. Differences in excitability of cortical neurons as a function of motor projection in conditioned cats. J. Neurophysiol. 36: 1104–1116, 1973.
 285. Woody, C. D., and G. Brozek. Gross potential from facial nucleus of cat as an index of neural activity in response to glabella tap. J. Neurophysiol. 32: 704–716, 1969.
 286. Woody, C. D., and G. Brozek. Changes in evoked responses from facial nucleus of cat with conditioning and extinction of an eye blink. J. Neurophysiol. 32: 717–726, 1969.
 287. Woody, C. D., and J. Engel, Jr. Changes in unit activity and threshold to electrical microstimulation at coronal‐pericruciate cortex of cat with classical conditioning of different facial movements. J. Neurophysiol. 35: 230–241, 1972.
 288. Woody, C. D., N. N. Vassilevsky, and J. Engle, Jr. Conditioned eye blink: unit activity at coronal‐precruciate cortex of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 33: 851–864, 1970.
 289. Yoon, M. G. Retention of the original topographic polarity by the 180† rotated tectal reimplant in adult goldfish. J. Physiol. London 233: 575–588, 1973.
 290. Younkin, S. G. The role of calcium in potentiation of the neuromuscular junction of frog sartorius. Proc. Ann. Meeting Soc. Neurosci., 2nd, Houston, Texas, 1972, p. 207.
 291. Zilber‐Gachelin, N. F., and M. P. Chartier. Modification of the motor reflex responses due to repetition of the peripheral stimulus in the cockroach. I. Habituation in the level of an isolated abdominal ganglion. J. Exptl. Biol. 59: 359–381, 1973.
 292. Zilber‐Gachelin, N. F., and M. P. Chartier. Modification of the motor reflex responses due to repetition of the peripheral stimulus in the cockroach. II. Conditions of activation of the motoneurones. J. Exptl. Biol. 59: 383–403, 1973.
 293. Zucker, R. S. Crayfish escape behavior and central synapses. I. Neural circuit exciting lateral giant fiber. J. Neurophysiol. 35: 599–620, 1972.
 294. Zucker, R. S. Crayfish escape behavior and central synapses. II. Physiological mechanisms underlying behavioral habituation. J. Neurophysiol. 35: 621–637, 1972.
 295. Zucker, R. S. Crayfish escape behavior and central synapses. III. Electrical junctions and dendrite spikes in fast flexor motoneurons. J. Neurophysiol. 35: 638–651, 1972.

Contact Editor

Submit a note to the editor about this article by filling in the form below.

* Required Field

How to Cite

Eric R. Kandel. Neuronal Plasticity and the Modification of Behavior. Compr Physiol 2011, Supplement 1: Handbook of Physiology, The Nervous System, Cellular Biology of Neurons: 1137-1182. First published in print 1977. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp010129