References |
1. |
Adams, J. A.
Human Memory.
New York:
McGraw‐Hill,
1967.
|
2. |
Adams, J. A.
Response feedback and learning.
Psychol. Bull.
70:
486–504,
1968.
|
3. |
Adams, J. A.
A closed‐loop theory of motor learning.
J. Mot. Behav.
3:
111–150,
1971.
|
4. |
Adams, J. A.,
Issues for a closed‐loop theory of motor learning.
In: Motor Control: Issues and Trends,
edited by
G. E. Stelmach.
New York:
Academic,
1976,
p. 87–107.
|
5. |
Adams, J. A.
Feedback theory of how joint receptors regulate the timing and positioning of a limb.
Psychol. Rev.
84:
504–523,
1977.
|
6. |
Adams, J. A., and
S. Dijkstra.
Short‐term memory for motor responses.
J. Exp. Psychol.
71:
314–318,
1966.
|
7. |
Allport, D. A.,
B. Antonis, and
P. Reynolds.
On the division of attention: a disproof of the single channel hypothesis.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
24:
225–235,
1972.
|
8. |
Angel, R. W.
Efference copy in the control of movement.
Neurology
26:
1164–1168,
1976.
|
9. |
Angel, R. W.,
H. Garland, and
M. Fischler.
Tracking errors amended without visual feedback.
J. Exp. Psychol.
89:
422–424,
1971.
|
10. |
Angel, R. W., and
J. R. Higgins.
Correction of false moves in pursuit tracking.
J. Exp. Psychol.
82:
185–187,
1969.
|
11. |
Bahrick, H. P.,
M. Noble, and
P. M. Fitts.
Extra‐task performance as a measure of learning a primary task.
J. Exp. Psychol.
48:
298–302,
1954.
|
12. |
Baker, C. H., and
P. Young.
Feedback during training and retention of motor skills.
Can. J. Psychol.
14:
257–264,
1960.
|
13. |
Barnes, G. R., and
M. A. Gresty.
Characteristics of eye movements to targets of short duration.
Aerosp. Med.
44:
1236–1240,
1973.
|
14. |
Bartlett, F. C.
Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology.
Cambridge:
Cambridge Univ. Press,
1932.
|
15. |
Bartz, A. E.
Fixation errors in eye movements to peripheral stimuli.
J. Exp. Psychol.
75:
444–446,
1967.
|
16. |
Bertelson, P.
Central intermittency twenty years later.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
18:
153–163,
1966.
|
17. |
Birmingham, H. P., and
F. V. Taylor.
A design philosophy for man‐machine control systems.
Proc. Inst. Radio Eng.
42:
1748–1758,
1954.
(In: Selected Papers on Human Factors in the Design and Use of Control Systems, edited by H. W. Sinaiko. New York: Dover, 1961, p. 67–87.).
|
18. |
Brand, J.
Classification without identification in visual search.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol:
23:
178–186,
1971.
|
19. |
Broadbent, D. E.
Perception and Communication.
Oxford:
Pergamon,
1958.
|
20. |
Brown, I. D.
Subjective and objective comparisons of successful and unsuccessful trainee drivers.
Ergonomics
9:
49–56,
1966.
|
21. |
Brown, I. D., and
E. C. Poulton.
Measuring the spare “mental capacity” of car drivers by a subsidiary task.
Ergonomics
4:
35–40,
1961.
|
22. |
Brown, I. D.,
A. H. Tickner, and
D. C. V. Simmonds.
Interference between concurrent tasks of driving and telephoning.
J. Appl. Psychol.
53:
419–424,
1969.
|
23. |
Brown, J.
Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
10:
12–21,
1958.
|
24. |
Buck, L.
Defining the boundary in a positioning task.
Acta Psychol.
42:
7–19,
1978.
|
25. |
Campbell, F. W., and
R. H. Wurtz.
Saccadic omission: why we do not see a grey‐out during a saccadic eye movement.
Vision Res.
18:
1297–1303,
1978.
|
26. |
Carlow, T.,
L. F. Dell'osso,
B. T. Troost,
R. B. Daroff, and
J. E. Birkett.
Saccadic eye movement latencies to multimodal stimuli.
Vision Res.
15:
1257–1262,
1975.
|
27. |
Carpenter, R. H. S.
Movements of the Eyes.
London:
Pion,
1977.
|
28. |
Chernikoff, R., and
M. Le May.
Effect of various display‐control configurations on tracking with identical and different coordinate dynamics.
J. Exp. Psychol.
66:
95–99,
1963.
|
29. |
Chernikoff, R., and
F. V. Taylor.
Reaction time to kinesthetic stimulation resulting from sudden arm displacement.
J. Exp. Psychol.
43:
1–8,
1952.
|
30. |
Conrad, R.
The Deaf School Child.
London:
Harper & Row,
1979.
|
31. |
Crago, P. E.,
J. C. Houk, and
Z. Hasan.
Regulatory actions of human stretch reflex.
J. Neurophysiol.
39:
925–935,
1976.
|
32. |
Craik, K. J. W.
Theory of the human operator in control systems. 1. The operator as an engineering system.
Br. J. Psychol.
38:
56–61,
1947.
|
33. |
Craik, K. J. W., and
M. A. Vince.
Psychological and physiological aspects of control mechanisms. II. With special reference to ground‐tank and A. A. tank gunnery.
Ergonomics
6:
419–440,
1963. (UK Med. Res. Counc. Mil. Personnel Res. Comm. Rep. BPC 44/322, London, 1944.).
|
34. |
Crossman, E. R. F. W.
A theory of the acquisition of speed‐skill.
Ergonomics
2:
153–166,
1959.
|
35. |
Davis, R.
Choice reaction times and the theory of intermittency in human performance.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
14:
157–166,
1962.
|
36. |
Davis, R.,
N. Moray, and
A. Treisman.
Imitative responses and the rate of gain of information.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
13:
78–89,
1961.
|
37. |
Desmedt, J. E., and
E. Godaux.
Ballistic skilled movements: load compensation and patterning of motor commands.
In: Progress in Clinical Neurophysiology. Cerebral Motor Control in Man: Long Loop Mechanisms,
edited by
J. E. Desmedt.
Basel:
Karger,
1978,
vol. 4,
p. 21–55.
|
38. |
Dickinson, J.
Adaptation level and retroactive interference in motor short‐term memory.
Percept. Mot. Skills
44:
691–694,
1977.
|
39. |
Diewert, G. L., and
E. A. Roy.
Coding strategy for memory of movement extent information.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem.
4:
666–675,
1978.
|
40. |
Elkind, J. I.
Characteristics of Simple Manual Control Systems.
Lexington:
MIT Lincoln Laboratory,
1956.
(Tech. Rep. 111.).
|
41. |
Ells, J. G.
Analysis of temporal and attentional aspects of movement control.
J. Exp. Psychol.
99:
10–21,
1973.
|
42. |
Ellson, D. G., and
L. Wheeler.
The Range Effect.
USAF Air Material Command, Dayton, OH:
Wright‐Patterson Air Force Base,
1949.
(Tech. Rep. 5813.).
|
43. |
Eriksen, C. W., and
J. S. Lappin.
Selective attention and very short‐term recognition memory for nonsense forms.
J. Exp. Psychol.
73:
358–364,
1967.
|
44. |
Evarts, E. V., and
R. Granit.
Relations of reflexes and intended movements.
In: Progress in Brain Research. Understanding the Stretch Reflex,
edited by
S. Homma.
Amsterdam:
Elsevier,
1976,
vol. 44,
p. 1–14.
|
45. |
Festinger, L., and
L. K. Canon.
Information about spatial location based on knowledge about efference.
Psychol. Rev.
72:
373–384,
1965.
|
46. |
Fitts, P. M., and
J. R. Peterson.
Information capacity of discrete motor responses.
J. Exp. Psychol.
67:
103–112,
1964.
|
47. |
Fitts, P. M., and
C. M. Seeger.
S‐R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes.
J. Exp. Psychol.
46:
199–210,
1953.
|
48. |
Fitts, P. M., and
G. Switzer.
Cognitive aspects of information processing: 1. The familiarity of S‐R sets and subsets.
J. Exp. Psychol.
63:
321–329,
1962.
|
49. |
Flowers, K.
Test of the ballistic hypothesis of movement and the ‘point of no return’.
Nature London
268:
174–175,
1977.
|
50. |
Garrity, L. I.
Electromyography: a review of the current status of subvocal speech research.
Mem. Cognit.
5:
615–622,
1977.
|
51. |
Gibbs, C. B.
Probability learning in step‐input tracking.
Br. J. Psychol.
56:
233–242,
1965.
|
52. |
Gordon, N. B.
Learning a motor task under varied display conditions.
J. Exp. Psychol.
57:
65–73,
1959.
|
53. |
Greenwald, A. G.
On doing two things at once: time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility.
J. Exp. Psychol.
94:
52–57,
1972.
|
54. |
Greenwald, A. G., and
H. G. Schulman.
On doing two things at once: II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect.
J. Exp. Psychol.
101:
70–76,
1973.
|
55. |
Gregory, R. L.
Eye movements and the stability of the visual world.
Nature London
182:
1214–1216,
1958.
|
56. |
Gundry, J.
The use of location and distance in reproducing different amplitudes of movement.
J. Mot. Behav.
7:
91–100,
1975.
|
57. |
Hagman, J. D.
Specific‐cue effects of interpolated movements on distance and location retention in short‐term motor memory.
Mem. Cognit.
6:
432–437,
1978.
|
58. |
Hagman, J. D., and
E. Williams.
Use of distance and location in short‐term motor memory.
Percept. Mot. Skills
44:
867–873,
1977.
|
59. |
Henson, D. B.
Corrective saccades: effects of altering visual feedback.
Vision Res.
18:
63–67,
1978.
|
60. |
Hick, W. E.
On the rate of gain of information.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
4:
11–26,
1952.
|
61. |
Higgins, J. R., and
R. W. Angel.
Correction of tracking errors without sensory feedback.
J. Exp. Psychol.
84:
412–416,
1970.
|
62. |
Ho, L., and
J. B. Shea.
Levels of processing and the coding of position cues in motor short‐term memory.
J. Mot. Behav.
10:
113–122,
1978.
|
63. |
Hollingworth, H. L.
The central tendency in judgment.
J. Philos. Psychol. Sci. Methods
7:
461–469,
1910.
|
64. |
Hyman, R.
Stimulus information as a determinant of reaction time.
J. Exp. Psychol.
45:
188–196,
1953.
|
65. |
Jaeger, R. J.,
G. C. Agarwal, and
G. L. Gottlieb.
Directional errors of movement and their correction in a discrete tracking task.
J. Mot. Behav.
11:
123–133,
1979.
|
66. |
Jones, B.
Outflow and inflow in movement duplication.
Percept. Psychophys.
12:
95–96,
1972.
|
67. |
Jones, B.
Role of central monitoring of efference in short‐term memory for movements.
J. Exp. Psychol.
102:
37–43,
1974.
|
68. |
Jones, B., and
M. R. Hulme.
Evidence for an outflow theory of skill.
Acta Psychol.
40:
49–56,
1976.
|
69. |
Kantowitz, B. H., and
J. L. Knight, Jr.
Testing tapping timesharing. II: auditory secondary task.
Acta Psychol.
40:
343–362,
1976.
|
70. |
Karlin, L., and
R. Kestenbaum.
Effects of number of alternatives on the psychological refractory period.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
20:
167–178,
1968.
|
71. |
Keele, S. W., and
J. G. Ells.
Memory characteristics of kinesthetic information.
J. Mot. Behav.
4:
127–134,
1972.
|
72. |
Keele, S. W., and
M. I. Posner.
Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements.
J. Exp. Psychol.
77:
155–158,
1968.
|
73. |
Kelso, J. A. S.
Motor control mechanisms underlying human movement reproduction.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.
3:
529–543,
1977.
|
74. |
Kelso, J. A. S.
Planning and efferent components in the coding of movement.
J. Mot. Behav.
9:
33–47,
1977.
|
75. |
Kelso, J. A. S., and
G. A. Frekany.
Coding processes in preselected and constrained movements: effect of vision.
Acta Psychol.
42:
145–161,
1978.
|
76. |
Kerr, B.
The effect of invalid task parameters on short‐term motor memory.
J. Mot. Behav.
10:
261–273,
1978.
|
77. |
Kowler, E., and
R. M. Steinman.
The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—I. Periodic target steps.
Vision Res.
19:
619–632,
1979.
|
78. |
Kowler, E., and
R. M. Steinman.
The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—II. Single target displacements.
Vision Res.
19:
633–646,
1979.
|
79. |
Laabs, G. J.
Retention characteristics of different reproduction cues in motor short‐term memory.
J. Exp. Psychol.
100:
168–177,
1973.
|
80. |
Laberge, D.,
R. Legrand, and
R. K. Hobbie.
Functional identification of perceptual and response biases in choice reaction time.
J. Exp. Psychol.
79:
295–299,
1969.
|
81. |
Leonard, J. A.
Tactual choice reactions: I.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
11:
76–83,
1959.
|
82. |
Lincoln, R. S.
Visual tracking: III. The instrumental dimension of motion in relation to tracking accuracy.
J. Appl. Psychol.
37:
489–493,
1953.
|
83. |
Mack, A.,
R. Fendrich, and
S. Sirigatti.
A rebound illusion in visual tracking.
Am. J. Psychol.
86:
425–433,
1973.
|
84. |
Marteniuk, R. G.
Retention characteristics of motor short‐term memory cues.
J. Mot. Behav.
5:
249–259,
1973.
|
85. |
Marteniuk, R. G.
Motor short‐term memory measures as a function of methodology.
J. Mot. Behav.
9:
247–250,
1977.
|
86. |
Marteniuk, R. G., and
E. A. Roy.
The codability of kinesthetic location and distance information.
Acta Psychol.
36:
471–479,
1972.
|
87. |
Marteniuk, R. G.,
K. W. Shields, and
S. Campbell.
Amplitude, position, timing and velocity as cues in reproduction of movement.
Percept. Mot. Skills
35:
51–58,
1972.
|
88. |
McLeod, P. D.
Inference of “attend to and learn” tasks with tracking.
J. Exp. Psychol.
99:
330–333,
1973.
|
89. |
McLeod, P.
A dual task response modality effect: support for multiprocessor models of attention.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
29:
651–667,
1977.
|
90. |
McLeod, P.
Parallel processing and the psychological refractory period.
Acta Psychol.
41:
381–396,
1977.
|
91. |
McLeod, P.
Does probe RT measure central processing demand?
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
30:
83–89,
1978.
|
92. |
McLeod, P., and
J. Meirop.
How to reduce manual response interference in the multiple task environment.
Ergonomics
22:
469–475,
1979.
|
93. |
McRuer, D. T.,
D. Graham, and
E. S. Krendel.
Manual control of single‐loop systems. Part I.
J. Franklin Inst.
283:
1–29,
1967.
|
94. |
McRuer, D. T.,
D. Graham, and
E. S. Krendel.
Manual control of single‐loop systems. Part II.
J. Franklin Inst.
283:
145–168,
1967.
|
95. |
Megaw, E. D.
Directional errors and their correction in a discrete tracking task.
Ergonomics
15:
633–643,
1972.
|
96. |
Megaw, E. D.,
Saccadic eye‐movements and the refractory period.
In: Attention and Performance V,
edited by
S. Kornblum.
London:
Academic,
1975,
p. 383–394.
|
97. |
Michael, J. A., and
G. M. Jones.
Dependence of visual tracking capability upon stimulus predictability.
Vision Res.
6:
707–716,
1966.
|
98. |
Moss, S. M.
Simple reaction time and response sets.
Hum. Factors
8:
231–244,
1966.
|
99. |
Mowbray, G. H., and
M. V. Rhoades.
On the reduction of choice reaction times with practice.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
11:
16–23,
1959.
|
100. |
Neisser, U.
Decision‐time without reaction‐time: experiments in visual scanning.
Am. J. Psychol.
76:
376–385,
1963.
|
101. |
Nisbett, R. E., and
T. De C. Wilson.
Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes.
Psychol. Rev.
84:
231–259,
1977.
|
102. |
Noble, M.,
P. M. Fitts, and
C. E. Warren.
The frequency response of skilled subjects in a pursuit tracking task.
J. Exp. Psychol.
49:
249–256,
1955.
|
103. |
Norman, D. A., and
D. G. Bobrow.
On data‐limited and resource‐limited processes.
Cognit. Psychol.
7:
44–64,
1975.
|
104. |
Peters, R. W.
The Effect of Low‐Pass Filtering of Sidetone Upon Speaker Intelligibility.
Pensacola, FL:
Naval Air Station, US Naval School of Aviation Medicine.
(Joint Project Rep. No. 49, April 1955.).
|
105. |
Peterson, L. R., and
M. J. Peterson.
Short‐term retention of individual verbal items.
J. Exp. Psychol.
58:
193–198,
1959.
|
106. |
Pew, R. W.,
Human perceptual‐motor performance.
In: Human Information Processing: Tutorials in Performance and Cognition,
edited by
B. H. Kantowitz.
New York:
Wiley,
1974,
p. 1–39.
|
107. |
Pew, R. W.,
J. C. Duffendack, and
L. K. Fensch.
Sine‐wave tracking revisited.
IEEE Trans. Hum. Factors Electron.
HFE8:
130–134,
1967.
|
108. |
Posner, M. I.
Characteristics of visual and kinesthetic memory codes.
J. Exp. Psychol.
75:
103–107,
1967.
|
109. |
Posner, M. I., and
S. J. Boies.
Components of attention.
Psychol. Rev.
78:
391–408,
1971.
|
110. |
Poulton, E. C.
Perceptual anticipation and reaction time.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
2:
99–112,
1950.
|
111. |
Poulton, E. C.
Perceptual Anticipation in Tracking.
Cambridge, England:
UK Med. Res. Counc. Appl. Psychol. Unit,
1950.
(Rep. No. 118.).
|
112. |
Poulton, E. C.
The basis of perceptual anticipation in tracking.
Br. J. Psychol.
43:
295–302,
1952.
|
113. |
Poulton, E. C.
On prediction in skilled movements.
Psychol. Bull.
54:
467–478,
1957.
|
114. |
Poulton, E. C.
On the stimulus and response in pursuit tracking.
J. Exp. Psychol.
53:
189–194,
1957.
|
115. |
Poulton, E. C.
Measuring the order of difficulty in visual‐motor tasks.
Ergonomics
1:
234–239,
1958.
|
116. |
Poulton, E. C.
The Medical Research Council's Applied Psychology Unit. Murmer.
Cambridge, England:
Cambridge Univ. Med. Soc.,
1964,
vol. 38,
p. 3–7.
|
117. |
Poulton, E. C.
Environment and Human Efficiency.
Springfield, IL:
Thomas,
1970.
|
118. |
Poulton, E. C.
Unwanted range effects from using within‐subject experimental designs.
Psychol. Bull.
80:
113–121,
1973.
|
119. |
Poulton, E. C.
Tracking Skill and Manual Control.
New York:
Academic,
1974.
|
120. |
Poulton, E. C.
Range effects in experiments on people.
Am. J. Psychol.
88:
3–32,
1975.
|
121. |
Poulton, E. C.
Composite model for human performance in continuous noise.
Psychol. Rev.
86:
361–375,
1979.
|
122. |
Poulton, E. C.
Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude.
Psychol. Bull.
86:
777–803,
1979.
|
123. |
Poulton, E. C., and
R. S. Edwards.
Asymmetric transfer in within‐subjects experiments on stress interactions.
Ergonomics
22:
945–961,
1979.
|
124. |
Poulton, E. C., and
P. R. Freeman.
Unwanted asymmetrical transfer effects with balanced experimental designs.
Psychol. Bull.
66:
1–8,
1966.
|
125. |
Prablanc, C., and
M. Jeannerod.
Corrective saccades: dependence on retinal reafferent signals.
Vision Res.
15:
465–469,
1975.
|
126. |
Prablanc, C.,
D. Massé, and
J. F. Echallier.
Error‐correcting mechanisms in large saccades.
Vision Res.
18:
557–560,
1978.
|
127. |
Reitman, J. S.
Mechanisms of forgetting in short‐term memory.
Cognit. Psychol.
2:
131–157,
1971.
|
128. |
Robinson, D. A.
Models of the saccadic eye movement control system.
Kybernetik
14:
71–83,
1973.
|
129. |
Roediger, H. L. III,
J. L. Knight, and
B. H. Kantowitz.
Inferring decay in short‐term memory: the issue of capacity.
Mem. Cognit.
5:
167–176,
1977.
|
130. |
Rosenbaum, D. A., and
O. Patashnik.
Time to time in the human motor system.
In: Attention and Performance VIII,
edited by
R. S. Nickerson.
Hillside, NJ:
Erlbaum,
1980.
|
131. |
Rosenthal, R.
Covert communication in the psychological experiment.
Psychol. Bull.
67:
356–367,
1967.
|
132. |
Roy, E. A., and
G. L. Diewert.
Encoding of kinesthetic extent information.
Percept. Psychophys.
17:
559–564,
1975.
|
133. |
Sanders, A. F.
Selective strategies in the assimilation of successively presented signals.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
16:
368–372,
1964.
|
134. |
Schmidt, R. A.
A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning.
Psychol. Rev.
82:
225–260,
1975.
|
135. |
Schmidt, R. A., and
G. B. Gordon.
Errors in motor responding, “rapid” corrections, and false anticipations.
J. Mot. Behav.
9:
101–111,
1977.
|
136. |
Schutz, R. W., and
E. A. Roy.
Absolute error: the devil in disguise.
J. Mot. Behav.
5:
141–153,
1973.
|
137. |
Shea, J. B.
Effects of labeling on motor short‐term memory.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem.
3:
92–99,
1977.
|
138. |
Shebilske, W. L.
Extraretinal information in corrective saccades and inflow vs outflow theories of visual direction constancy.
Vision Res.
16:
621–628,
1976.
|
139. |
Shiffrin, R. M.
Information persistence in short‐term memory.
J. Exp. Psychol.
100:
39–49,
1973.
|
140. |
Slack, C. W.
Learning in simple one‐dimensional tracking.
Am. J. Psychol.
66:
33–44,
1953.
|
141. |
Slack, C. W.
Some characteristics of the “range effect.”
J. Exp. Psychol.
46:
76–80,
1953.
|
142. |
Smith, M. C.
Theories of the psychological refractory period.
Psychol. Bull.
67:
202–213,
1967.
|
143. |
Spelke, E.,
W. Hirst, and
U. Neisser.
Skills of divided attention.
Cognition
4:
215–230,
1976.
|
144. |
Stark, L.,
G. Vossius, and
L. R. Young.
Predictive control of eye tracking movements.
IEEE Trans. Hum. Factors Electron.
HFE3:
52–57,
1962.
|
145. |
Stelmach, G. E.
Kinesthetic recall and information reduction activity.
J. Mot. Behav.
2:
183–194,
1970.
|
146. |
Stelmach, G. E.,
J. A. S. Kelso, and
P. D. McCullagh.
Preselection and response biasing in short‐term motor memory.
Mem. Cognit.
4:
62–66,
1976.
|
147. |
Stelmach, G. E.,
J. A. S. Kelso, and
S. A. Wallace.
Preselection in short‐term motor memory.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. Mem.
1:
745–755,
1975.
|
148. |
Stelmach, G. E., and
M. Wilson.
Kinesthetic retention, movement extent, and information processing.
J. Exp. Psychol.
85:
425–430,
1970.
|
149. |
Sternberg, S.
High‐speed scanning in human memory.
Science
153:
652–654,
1966.
|
150. |
Trumbo, D., and
F. Milone.
Primary task performance as a function of encoding, retention, and recall in a secondary task.
J. Exp. Psychol.
91:
273–279,
1971.
|
151. |
Trumbo, D.,
M. Noble,
F. Fowler, and
J. Porterfield.
Motor performance on temporal tasks as a function of sequence length and coherence.
J. Exp. Psychol.
77:
397–406,
1968.
|
152. |
Vince, M. A.
Some Exceptions to the Psychological Refractory Period in Unskilled Manual Responses.
Cambridge, England:
UK Med. Res. Counc. Appl. Psychol. Unit,
1950.
(Rep. No. 124.).
|
153. |
Watkins, M. J.,
O. C. Watkins,
F. I. M. Craik, and
G. Mazuryk.
Effect of non‐verbal distraction on short‐term storage.
J. Exp. Psychol.
101:
296–300,
1973.
|
154. |
Way, T. C., and
R. Gottsdanker.
Psychological refractoriness with varying differences between tasks.
J. Exp. Psychol.
78:
38–45,
1968.
|
155. |
Weiss, B.
Movement error, pressure variation, and the range effect.
J. Exp. Psychol.
50:
191–196,
1955.
|
156. |
Welford, A. T.
The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high‐speed performance— a review and a theory.
Br. J. Psychol.
43:
2–19,
1952.
|
157. |
Welford, A. T.
Evidence for a single‐channel decision mechanism limiting performance in a serial reaction task.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
11:
193–210,
1959.
|
158. |
Welford, A. T.
Single‐channel operation in the brain.
Acta Psychol.
27:
5–22,
1967.
|
159. |
Wheeless, L. L., Jr.,
R. M. Boynton, and
G. H. Cohen.
Eye‐movement responses to step and pulse‐step stimuli.
J. Opt. Soc. Am.
56:
956–960,
1966.
|
160. |
Woodworth, R. S.
Experimental Psychology.
New York:
Holt,
1938.
|